This research specifically incorporates the impact of the disposition effect of trading volume into the model construction to more accurately verify the overconfidence behavior of investors in the Taiwan stock market. Based on the theoretical framework and related literature of overconfidence and disposition behavior, we propose the hypothesis to be test and further uses Taiwan stock price index with a turnover rate and return rate after removing the weekly effect for about ten-year data, and construct a tri-variate VAR-GARCH model to examine the investor overconfidence behavior and disposition effect of Taiwan stock market. The empirical results indicate that investors in the Taiwan stock market do have a tendency to overconfidence and are easily affected by the market environment. According to the risk of market capitalization, overconfident investors are not necessarily inclined to hold higher risk stocks. In addition to overconfidence behavior, investors in the Taiwan stock market also have a significant disposition effect, indicating that investors cannot rationally judge the timing of selling or purchasing their stocks. By recognizing this relationship and analyzing the biased trends from overconfidence and disposition effect in trading volumes and returns, such knowledge can help the investors/practitioners to develop strategies and take appropriate measures.
In 1973, there were only 62 listed companies on the Taiwan stock market, and the amount of total annual stock transaction about NT$87 billion, but by the end of 2019, the number of listed companies has grown more than tenfold to a total of 942 companies, and the total annual transaction amount is as high as NT$26.37 trillion (US$86.50 billion) (Source: Taiwan Stock Exchange). According to these data, the Taiwan stock market is growing rapidly, and the amount of investment injected into the stock market is also increasing day by day. With the growth of the stock market, the number of investors participating in the stock market has also increased, and the irrational investment behaviors of investors have become more and more obvious, such as investors chasing ups and downs, weekend effects, and even the so-called "pre-election market", etc. Prove that investors are not completely rational. In addition, apart from the traditional hedging and liquidity requirements, the huge trading volume today is believed to be partly attributable to investors’ overconfidence behavior or disposition effect. This is what we will explore the issues in this study.
Human beings are often overconfident in their abilities, knowledge and future prospects. Odean 1 showed that overconfident investors trade more than rational investors, which reduces their expected utility. Overconfidence leads to more trading and lower expected utility. When investors have overconfidence, their investment behavior will become more positive (aggressive) due to past investment profits, and due to overestimation of self-ability, that is, self-attribution bias, it is easy to cause to neglect the risk, holding too many high-risk stocks, or because of too many transactions, the profit of investment is eroded by transaction costs and reduced, these are all the damages caused by overconfidence to investors (Statman, Thorley and Vorkink 2; Chuang and Lee 3; Ho 4 and Liu, Chuang, Huang and Chen 5). Therefore, by examining the overconfidence in the Taiwan stock market, it can remind Taiwan stock investors to avoid this irrational behavior to reduce unnecessary losses. This is also the main research motivation of this research.
The disposition effect pointed out by Shefrin and Statman 6 is that investors tend to continue to hold stocks that are currently losing money and sell stocks with capital gains due to psychological factors such as self-esteem or fear of regret. The disposition effect is reflected in the stock market trading, which will produce a reaction similar to the effect of overconfidence. Investors who have the disposition effect will increase their trading volume due to the increase in return of individual stocks. The excessive trading volume in the stock market may not only be affected by investors' overconfidence, but we cannot rule out the possibility of being affected by the disposition effect. In addition, when overconfidence is tested by transaction volume, if the effect of the sanction is not taken into consideration, the transaction volume caused by the disposition effect will be mistakenly attributed to the research bias caused by overconfidence. Therefore, this research is necessary to separate the overconfidence from the transaction caused by the disposition effect, and to verify the overconfidence and the disposition effect at the same time.
Based on the above discussions, in order to explore the effect of overconfidence and disposition effect, this research first verifies the tendency of overconfidence with the return rate and turnover rate of the overall market. Then, uses market returns, individual stock returns and individual stock turnover rates to simultaneously test the overconfidence and the disposition effect. Hopefully, the empirical results for the overconfidence behavior and disposition effect of investors in this study can provide suggestions for the investment decision in the Taiwan stock market.
The remaining of the study is organized as follows. Section 2 reviews the necessary literature which also provides the theoretical background for the concepts of overconfidence and disposition effects. Section 3 briefly describes the research hypotheses and model setups in this study. Section 4 discusses the empirical results and analysis. Section 5 presents the concluding remarks.
As indicated by many literature, investors often exhibit the two main behavioral biases of overconfidence and disposition effects and may make poor trading decisions in the stock market. There is extensive research on specific biases that influence investor behavior. For the purpose of this study, we only focus on overconfidence and disposition effects. Given that investors often make two mistakes: (i) excessive trading caused by overconfidence, and (ii) stock traders tend to sell winners and hold losers in a trading pattern, when disposing of stocks, which is the so-called disposition effect (Barber and Odean 7; Chen, Kim, Nofsinger and Rui 8; Siwar 9; Ben-David and Hirshleifer 10; Prosad, Kapoor, Sengupta and Roychoudhary 11; Bhatia and Sharma 12 and Trejos, van Deemen, Rodríguez and Gomez 13). In this section, the necessary literature review provides the theoretical background for the concepts of overconfidence and the disposition effect, and attempts to link these two biases. Especially, this section reviews these two concepts and reports on recent research related to them. In particular, this section reviews these two concepts and reports on recent research related to them.
The theory of overconfidence is one of the important theories in behavior finance. In this subject, the study of overconfidence behaviors such as self-attribution bias has gradually received attention in recent years. More and more researches studying this research believe that, compared with the traditional efficient market hypothesis (Fama 14, 15), the trading behavior caused by overconfidence and the phenomenon reflected in the price-volume relationship are closer to the real human behavior. It also better reflects the performance of the real market.
People’s behavior indicates that they are stronger than they actually are, which is called overconfidence in psychology (Chen, Kim, Nofsinger, and Rui 8). The concept of overconfidence originated from a large number of cognitive psychology experiments and surveys, in which subjects overestimated their predictive ability and the accuracy of the information provided (Yates 16; Campbell, Goodie and Foster 17; Zaiane and Abaoub 18; Ouarda and El Bori 19 and Mushinada and Veluri 20). People do not calibrate well in estimating the probability of events they think will happen. In short, people think they have better information than they actually are. Overconfidence does not necessarily mean people are ignorant or incompetent. Rather, people think that their judgments and estimates of the situation are better than the actual situation (Pompian 21). Some studies have shown that investors tend to be overconfident in their financial decisions (Fischhoff, Slovic and Lichtenstein 22; Statman, Thorley and Vorkink, 2; Chuang and Lee 3; Zaiane and Abaoub 18; Ouarda and El Bori 19 and Mushinada and Veluri 20).
Since the increase in investment market volatility in recent years is often accompanied by violent fluctuations in trading volume, it can also be guessed that investors in the market are often stimulated by the previous rewards and losses, resulting in more active or inactive transactions in the next period. Once the stock price rises violently, investors’ behaviors such as overconfidence, over-optimism, and self-attribution bias will appear at any time. Therefore, it is understood that part of the fluctuations in the trading volume in the market may be associated with the phenomenon caused by investors' overconfidence. The research of Barber and Odean 7, 23, 24 used detailed stock trading data of individual investors as samples, and empirically found that retail investors have a clear tendency to overconfidence. Because these investors in the sample will adopt a positive attitude to trade stocks, and actively trading stocks did not bring higher return on investment for these investors. In addition, from the perspective of the entire market, Statman, Thorley, Vorkink 2, Chuang and Lee 3, Ho 4 and Liu, Chuang, Huang and Chen 5 also found that most investors have overconfidence, so overconfidence is the majority of investment People's unavoidable systematic cognitive bias. Overconfident traders believe that the private information they possess is more correct than the information revealed. Stock market trading volume and stock market volatility will increase due to the degree of investor overconfidence. Investors will trade more frequently, but their performance is worse than before. When investors’ confidence increases, investors will trade riskier stocks. They believe this is due to the overconfidence of these investors.
In addition, Shefrin and Statman 6 mentioned the deposition effect. They pointed out that sometimes investors will choose to continue to hold the loss-making portfolio due to psychological factors such as self-esteem or fear of regret and sell the profitable part. In stock trading, the deposition effect often produces situations similar to the effect of overconfidence (Kadous, Tayler, Thayer and Young 25). Excessive trading volume in the stock market may be affected by overconfidence. Investors with deposition effect may also increase trading volume due to the profit and loss of their investment portfolio. We cannot be sure that the abnormal fluctuations in trading volume are entirely due to overconfidence. Therefore, when testing overconfidence by trading volume, if the deposition effect is not taken into consideration, it may mistakenly attribute the fluctuation of the amount caused by the deposition effect to the bias caused by overconfidence.
Odean 1 and Frazzini 26 found that traders usually hold the losers too long and sell the winners too early. Grinblatt and Keloharju 27 also presented the Finnish investors wouldn’t like to realize the losses. Cerqueira Leal, Armada and Duque 28 found the strong evidence of the disposition effect on the Portuguese stock market, and the disposition effect is stronger in bull market than in bear market. Barber and Odean 29 and Kadous, Tayler, Thayer and Young 25 found that lower self-regard (and/or higher confidence) investors will hold losing investments longer than higher self-regard (and/or lower confidence). Heimer 30 examined the relationships between the social interaction and disposition effect, and find that individuals will have doubles disposition effect if they contact the social interactions. Komai, Koyano and Miyakawa 31 estimated the investors’ trading activities accounting for buying and selling stocks conditional on the observed returns of Japanese stocks. The results indicate that the individual investors make contrarian trades, i.e., tend to buy stocks exhibiting lower past return. In order to understand the disposition effect of bonds, which are as strong as stocks, Hincapie-Salazar and Agudelo 32 applied Odean's 33 measurements to proprietary trading databases with unique investor IDs from emerging market exchanges that trade both stocks and bonds. They found that bonds have a certain disposition effect, but they are much lower than stocks, and a positive relation between the two measures by investor. In addition, they also show that local individuals and family offices in these two markets have significant disposition effects. In contrast, long-term institutions, brokerage companies and foreign investors do not show this biased trends.
The theory of overconfidence and the deposition effect can be seen at the earliest by the prospect theory proposed by Kahneman and Tverskey 34. This argument explains why independent individuals prefer risk in some situations, but they do not at other times. Retention of risk is a contradiction that traditional expected utility theory cannot explain. In addition, the theory also points out that people have cognitive biases, which are not completely rational as described in traditional theories. That is, when people face decisions, they often use past experience or intuition to make judgments (De Bondt and Thaler 35). Take the overconfidence of investors as an example. Usually when the stock price soars after the investor buys a stock, the investor will think that his judgment is correct and he has thought about it, so he buys a profitable stock. However, in hindsight, the investment positions that investors often buy are relatively risky, which often results in the dilemma that the initial profits are not enough to make up for the current losses (Trejos, van Deemen and Gomez 13). The proposal of the prospect theory has indeed produced a considerable degree of incentive effect for researches who want to find the best explanation for the abnormal phenomenon, and then gradually changed from the perspective of investor irrational for further discussion, hoping to find out what people will deal with. Decision-making is more truly reflected in the behavioral pattern of market investment amount or transaction volume. Among them, Shefrin 36 mainly divides the subject of this school into three categories, each of which is heuristic-driven bias, frame dependence and inefficient markets. The overconfidence discussed in this belongs to the category of heuristic-driven bias. Recently, Khan, Afeef, Adil and Ullah 37 attempted to detect the behavioral factors affecting the investment decisions of institutional investors in the asset management industry of Pakistan through partial least squares structural equation model (PLS-SEM). The empirical results show that institutional investors are significantly tend to behavioral biases, such as disposition effects, overconfidence biases, psychological accounting, and diversification biases. These findings signify that institutional investors in Pakistan are not entirely rational economic agents. Instead, behavioral and personality factors correlate with their investment decisions, making them bounded rational.
The research hypotheses and model setups corresponding to the hypotheses in this study are discussed in this section.
3.1. Research hypothesesBased on above related theory and literature review, this study set ups two hypotheses that mainly consider the concept of Chuang and Lee 3 on investor overconfidence, using Odean 1, 33 and Gervais and Odean 38 to verify overconfidence by testing inter-temporal changes in transaction volume, and also incorporate the disposition effects pointed out by Statman, Thorley, and Vorkink 2 to construct a complete model to conduct an empirical analysis of the overconfidence and disposition effect of investors in the Taiwan stock market. The two main hypothesis of this research is described as follows:
Hypothesis 1(H1): Investors still have overconfidence after verifying the influence of overconfidence and disposition effects on trading volumes at the same time.
Hypothesis 2(H2): Overconfidence investors tend to underestimate the risks and hold the riskier stocks due to their too much faith in their stock selection ability and trade too many high-risk stocks.
In accordance with the four hypotheses mentioned above, this study uses the tri-variate VAR-GARCH model to examine the overconfidence behavior and deposition effect of investors in the Taiwan stock market. These will be discussed later.
3.2. Model SetupsThe models corresponding to the above hypotheses in this study are constructed as follows:
To test the hypothesis H1, this study uses a tri-variate vector autoregressive (VAR) with GARCH (tri-variate VAR-GARCH) model.
1. Conditional Mean Equation
![]() | (1) |
![]() | (2) |
![]() | (3) |
2. Conditional Variance Equation
![]() | (4) |
![]() | (5) |
![]() | (6) |
3. Conditional Co-variance Equation
![]() | (7) |
![]() | (8) |
![]() | (9) |
![]() |
Where
: Individual stock turnover at time t
: Individual stock return at time t
: Market return at time t
: Stock return volatility at time t
: Rresidual terms of conditional mean equations at time t.
: Intercept terms of conditional mean equations
: Coefficients of delayed individual stock turnover
: Coefficient of delayed individual stock return returns
: Coefficient of delayed market returns
: Coefficient of
: Conditional variance of
: Conditional variance of
: Conditional variance of
: Conditional co-variance of
and
: Conditional co-variance of
and
: Conditional co-variance of
and
: Intercept terms of conditional variance equations
: GARCH effects of
,
and
in conditional variance equations
: ARCH effects of
,
and
in conditional variance equations
: Intercept terms of conditional co-variance equations
: Measure the effects of cross volatility
on co-variance of each two variables about
,
and
.
: Measure the effects of cross shock
on co-variance of each two variables about
,
and
.
The above VAR-GARCH type model established by this study can overcome the doubts about the identification of endogenous and exogenous variables, and can show the influence of any variable on all other variables. Therefore, this study uses the tri-variate VAR-GARCH model, which treats both individual stock return rates turnover
and market return rates
as endogenous variables, and stock price fluctuations
is also considered since the model will be applied to each stock individually, it does not need to be spread over stock returns. The reason for taking
as an exogenous variable is based on Karpoff’s 39 research and used to examine the contemporaneous relationship between individual stock trading volume (turnover), individual stock return, market return and its volatility in this study. Ross 40 indicated that in a market that lacks friction (no arbitrage opportunities), the proportion of information flow can be observed by the degree of price fluctuations. Therefore, this study uses
variable to take into account price fluctuations of transactions caused by information from other firms or market. This exogenous variable that controls fluctuations is similar to the mean absolute deviation (MAD) used by Bessembinder, Chan and Seguin 41 and follow the setting of French, Schwert and Stambaugh 42 to represent the firm-specific information flow that affects market transactions. The calculation method of week
used in this study is as follows:
![]() | (10) |
Where the stock return on day t, and T is the number of trading days in the week.
As for the selection of the most suitable lag term of the VAR model, after detecting from the ADF unit-root test that according to the AIC criterion, this study selects lag 3 periods as the optimal number of lag periods. We also find that there is a heterogeneous variance in the estimated errors of these three equations by ARCH-LM test. Then, tri-variate VAR-GARCH is used to estimate and discusses the interactive relationship among individual stock return individual stock turnover
and market return
to examine and identify the overconfidence behavior and deposition effect of investors in the Taiwan stock market.
According to the results of model estimation, if we find significantly positive, it means that investors will receive an increase in the return of individual stocks and tend to sell individual stocks, which means that investors have the disposition effect. If
is significantly positive, it means that investors will be motivated by market profits and their investment confidence will increase, and they will trade more actively in the following period, that is, investors have overconfidence. If
and
are both positive and significant, it means that in this stock, we observe that investors have both disposition effect and overconfidence behavior.
For testing Hypothesis H2, we regard the size of the company as the degree of risk of the company, that is, the risk of large companies is smaller, and the risk of small companies is greater. The size of the company is measured by the average market value during the sample period. After averaging the daily market value of all companies during the sample period, rank the companies with the top 50 market capitalization as a low-risk portfolio, and the smallest market capitalization 50 Companies are considered as high-risk investment portfolios. Under the above model for verification and based on the theory of overconfidence, this study expects that in companies with small market capitalization, the overconfidence will be stronger than companies with large market capitalization.
Before conducting empirical analysis, it is necessary to describe all the research data and determine the appropriate measurement model based on the type of data. In order to facilitate the subsequent research steps and obtain more accurate empirical results. This section first introduces the source and selection of research data, the data processing, and then explains the results of unit-root test as well as ARCH-LM test for VAR model residuals.
(1) Data Sources and Processing
The aim of this research is to explore whether there is overconfidence behavior and disposition effect in the Taiwan stock market. The Taiwan stock market index and the stock price data of listed companies are used as the research objects. In terms of Taiwan stock market index, this study extracts the daily data of the Taiwan Economic Journal (TEJ) from January 8, 1998 (Thursday) to December 26, 2007 (Wednesday), this period is chosen to avoid the abnormal influences of rare occurrences of less special events, and to clearly examine overconfidence and disposition effect. About ten years of ex-dividend adjustment, the daily data of the market index constructs the market weekly return and turnover rate required by this research. In addition to avoiding the situation that certain stocks have zero daily trading volume, the weekly data used in this study mainly considers the speed of overconfidence reactions. After the investor obtains the profit, there is usually a slight delay before the next transaction. This study uses the weekly data to carry out empirical analysis.
In addition, with the development of the Taiwan stock market, the number of listed companies has continued to increase, and the total number of listed shares has also increased. If the transaction volume is simply used to measure the amount of transactions, it will inevitably be affected by the increase in the number of shares, and the data will lose its accuracy. Therefore, this research uses turnover rate to represent the amount of transactions to avoid this situation. In terms of stock price information of listed companies, the selected data period is the same as that of the market index. This research first selects 321 listed companies that have been listed for trading since January 8, 1998 and are still in the centralized market as of December 26, 2007. Then, in order to ensure the consistency of the sample length, if the 321 stocks have stopped trading during the sample period (for example, UMC (company code 2303)) reduced its capital on September 20, 2007 and ceased its listing until October 9, 2007. Weekly observations that do not match the market (506 observations in total) will also be eliminated. After this procedure, a total of 263 stocks are eligible. This study uses the data of 263 stocks as the stock price of the listed company data. In order to avoid the implied weekly effect of the turnover rate and the rate of return from causing errors in the estimation results, the turnover rate and the weekly rate of return in this study are both calculated starting from Thursday and accumulating to the next Wednesday. Returns of stock at time t, is defined as:
and
denotes the weekly stock price at time t.
(2) Results of Unit-Root Test and ARCH-LM Test for VAR Model Residuals
The augmented Dickey-Fuller (ADF) and Phillips-Perron (PP) tests are used to test for unit roots. The results reported in Table 1 shows that both the ADF and the PP models are greater than their respective critical values of individual stock return individual stock turnover
and market return
This suggests that the unit root hypothesis is rejected for individual stock return
individual stock turnover
and market return
None of the data series for the three variables exhibit the unit root. It shows that there are stationary series.
Based on the results of the ADF and PP unit-root tests, it can be determined that the individual stock turnover individual stock return
and market return
used in this study are all stationary series. Therefore, the vector autoregressive (VAR) model (Equations (1), (2) and (3)) can be used instead of the vector error correction model (VECM). We also find that there is a heterogeneous variance in the estimated errors of these three conditional mean equations by ARCH-LM test (Table 2). Then, tri-variate VAR-GARCH (Equations (1), (2)… and (9)) is used to estimate and discusses the interactive relationship among individual stock return
individual stock turnover
and market return
to examine and identify the overconfidence behavior and deposition effect of investors in the Taiwan stock market.
Based on the empirical results of tri-variate VAR-GARCH model, we can verify the research hypotheses in this study.
(1) Verification of Overconfidence Behavior and Disposition Effect
Hypothesis 1(H1): Investors still have overconfidence after verifying the influence of overconfidence and disposition effects on trading volumes at the same time.
The tri-variate VAR-GARCH (Equations (1), (2)… and (9)) constructed in this study can simultaneously estimate the impact of the disposition effect and the overconfidence effect on the turnover rate. The coefficients of tri-variate VAR-GARCH model is estimated by quasi-maximum likelihood estimation (QMLE), the maximum likelihood estimates of the parameters are obtained by numerical maximization of the log-likelihood function using the Marquardt 43 algorithm, which is an updated modification of the well-known BHHH method of Berndt, Hall, Hall, Hausman 44. Based on the estimated results, when the turnover of individual stocks is significantly positively affected by the return of individual stocks in the previous period (, k=1, 2 and 3), which means that individual stock investors have a disposition effect. When the turnover of individual stocks is significantly positively affected by the previous market return (
, k=1, 2 and 3), it means that individual stock investors have an overconfidence effect. On these basis, this study verifies the selected individual stocks whether investors have disposition effects and overconfidence effects.
It should be noted that due to the large number of individual stocks selected, the estimation results of the individual stocks are not listed one by one, but the estimation results and the disposition effect and the overconfidence effect are really verified. This research divides the empirical results into four categories, namely (1) Stocks with overconfidence effects only. (2) Stocks with disposition effects only. (3) Stocks with overconfidence effects and disposition effects at the same time. (4) Neither overconfidence effect nor disposition effects. Here, only one stock (represented by company code) is selected to show the results for each category and indicated in Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 for proving each results. In order to check the adequacy of the model, in this study we used the Ljung-Box Q test to calculate the standardized residuals and squared residuals and cross-product of standardized residuals. As indicated in Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6, all results of Ljung-Box Q tests (Q(12), Q(18) and Q(24)) fail to reject the null hypothesis that the estimated standardized residuals , squared standardized residuals
and cross-product of the two standardized residuals
have no autocorrelation and ARCH effects. Therefore, these above tests verify that the VAR-GARCH model we estimated is an appropriate specification for the relationships among individual stock turnover (
), individual stock return (
) and market return (
) and the empirical results are also useful and applicable.
The company codes of stocks that only have the disposition effects and that only have the effect of overconfidence are listed in Table 7. According to the statistical results, it is observed that there are 110 stocks that only have the disposition effect, accounting for 41.985% of the selected stock sample; the stocks that only have the overconfidence effect have a total of 33 stocks, accounting for 12.595% of the selected stock sample. There are obviously more stocks that only have the disposition effect than those that only have the effect of overconfidence.
The company codes of stocks that have both the disposition effect and the overconfidence effect, and the stocks that do not have the disposition effect or the overconfidence effect are listed in Table 8. Among them, there are a total of 110 stocks that have both the disposition effect and the overconfidence effect, which is the same as the stocks that only have the disposition effect, accounting for 41.985% of the selected stock sample; there are only 9 stocks that have no disposition effect or overconfidence effect, accounting for 3.435% of the selected stock samples. It means that almost all of the stocks in the Taiwan stock market have disposition effects or overconfidence effects that affect the individual stock turnover.
Throughout this research sample selection of individual stocks, there are 220 stocks with disposition effects (only 110 stocks with disposition effects plus 110 stocks with both disposition effects and overconfidence effects), accounting for 83.97% of the selected stock samples. There are a total of 143 stocks with overconfidence effect (only 33 stocks with overconfidence effect plus 110 stocks with both overconfidence and disposition effects), accounting for 54.58% of the selected stock samples. In the presence of both the disposition and overconfidence effects, overconfidence behavior still has influence on stock trading of investors. Hypothesis 1(H1) is accepted, i.e., investors still have overconfidence behavior after verifying the influence of overconfidence and disposition effects on trading volumes at the same time.
Based on the above empirical results, it can be found that in the selected individual stock samples, the disposition effect on individual stock turnover is quite significant, which means that most stock investors have a tendency to sell stocks with current profits, but hold off selling stocks with current losses. This study also found that there are more than half of the individual stocks whose turnover will be significantly positively affected by market returns. That is, it is observed that the investors in these stocks are overconfident due to the increase in the market returns, and will trade more actively in the following period. Therefore, after considering both the effect of disposition and of overconfidence on the individual stock turnover, although the effect of overconfidence on the turnover of individual stocks is not as significant as the effect of the disposition effect, most individual stocks can also observe that investors have overconfidence in trading. This means that investors still have overconfidence after considering the disposition effect on the individual stock turnover in Taiwan stock market.
(2) Verification of Company Risk and Overconfidence Trading
Hypothesis 2(H2): Overconfidence investors tend to underestimate the risks and hold the riskier stocks due to their too much faith in their stock selection ability and trade too many high-risk stocks.
Regarding Hypothesis 2(H2) in this study, the average market value of individual stock companies calculated by the data within the sample period is used as the standard to measure the risk of individual stocks. When the average market value of individual stocks is larger, the risk of the company is greater. When the average market value of individual stocks is smaller, the risk of this company is smaller. After ranking the selected stocks in order of their average market capitalization, the top 50 companies with market capitalization are regarded as low-risk portfolios while the 50 companies with the smallest market capitalization are regarded as high-risk portfolios. In this study, the disposition and overconfidence effects of high-risk and low-risk stocks are verified, and the results are summarized in Table 9.
Based on the statistical results of overconfidence and disposition effects of high-risk and low-risk individual stocks (Table 9), we find that among the low-risk individual stocks, there are 33 stocks with disposition effects, accounting for 66% of low-risk stocks, and 28 stocks with overconfidence effects, accounting for 56% of low-risk stocks, without disposition or overconfidence effects. There are 6 stocks with effect, accounting for 12% of the sample of low-risk stocks. In the part of high-risk stocks, there are 25 stocks with disposition effects, accounting for 50% of high-risk stocks, and 28 stocks with overconfidence effects, accounting for 56% of high-risk stocks, with no disposition or overconfidence effect. There is only one stock of stocks, accounting for 2% of the sample of high-risk stocks.
According to the empirical evidences shown in Table 9, among high-risk stocks, the situation of having disposition effect is more obvious than that of low-risk stocks and more high-risk stocks have disposition effect than those with overconfidence effect. Among the low-risk stocks, however, there are slightly more stocks with overconfidence effect than those with disposition effect. There are 28 stocks with overconfidence effects in both high-risk stocks and low-risk stocks. High-risk stocks have an overconfidence effect assumed by this research is not true. Therefore, the Hypothesis 2(H2) of this study is not accepted for the situation where low-risk stocks have an overconfidence effect will be less obvious than when high-risk stocks have an overconfidence effect. In this study, under the risk classification standard of individual stock market value, overconfident investors will not obviously tend to trade or hold high-risk (small-market value) stocks. However, it is worth noting that among the low-risk stocks, 6 stocks have no overconfidence effect or disposition effect (Table 9), which is obviously more than high-risk stocks. This means that compared with high-risk stock investors, low-risk stock investors are less likely to have irrationality such as overconfidence behavior or disposition effect. Then, this research can reasonably infer that more rational investors will tend to invest in low-risk (large market capitalization) individual stocks, rather than investing in high-risk (small market capitalization) of individual stocks.
Given that the disposition effect has a similar impact on trading volume as overconfidence, moreover most previous studies have ignored the disposition effect when studying the overconfidence behavior. Therefore, the main purpose of this research is to examine both the overconfidence behavior and disposition effect of investors in the Taiwan stock market. The main findings and conclusions are indicated as follows:
Considering both the effect of disposition and of overconfidence on the individual stock turnover, although the effect of overconfidence on the turnover of individual stocks ism less significant than the disposition effect, most individual stocks can also observe that investors have overconfidence in trading. This means that investors are still overconfident after considering the disposition effect on the individual stock turnover in Taiwan stock market.
In this study, under the risk classification standard of individual stock market value, overconfident investors will not obviously tend to hold high-risk (small-market value) stocks. Compared with high-risk stock investors, low-risk stock investors are less likely to have irrationality such as overconfidence behavior or disposition effect. Then, the study can reasonably infer that more rational investors will tend to invest in low-risk (large market capitalization) individual stocks, rather than investing in high-risk (small market capitalization) of individual stocks.
It is worth noting that the results from our two hypothesis tests, the results give us some senses that under the disposition effect on the individual stock turnover, investors still have overconfidence in the overall data period, which means that investors will overestimate their own capabilities and even own the private information they possess because of their investment profits. In order to continue to maintain this confidence, investors will choose to sell profitable stocks as soon as possible to prove that the profit comes from their own ability, but are reluctant to realize the loss in order to avoid admitting their mistakes in judgment.
In sum, the main findings of this research can help investors/practitioners identify biased trends from overconfidence and/or disposition effect in advance and make investment/hedge strategies accordingly, i.e., such knowledge can help the investors/practitioners to develop strategies and take appropriate measures. The limitation of the study is that the sample in this study is selected for ten years, so the eligible stocks are 262 samples. Future study can consider selecting different sample periods and number of stocks. The results obtained can be verified with this research.
Finally, for this research, it is necessary to recognize the conclusions and suggestions put forward in the text, which are based on the relevant research methods, actual data and empirical model cited in this study. When the follow-up researchers refer to and cite, it is advisable to consider the changes in the time and space environment during the research period, so that it can be applied flexibly in actual situations.
[1] | Odean, T. (1998a). Volume, volatility, price, and profit when all traders are above average. The Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1887-1934. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[2] | Statman, M., Thorley, S. and Vorkink, K. (2006). Investor overconfidence and trading volume. The Review of Financial Studies, 19(4), 1531-1565. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[3] | Chuang, W. I. and Lee, B. S. (2006). An empirical evaluation of the overconfidence hypothesis. Journal of Banking and Finance, 30(9), 2489-2515. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[4] | Ho, C. M. (2011). Does overconfidence harm individual investors? An empirical analysis of the Taiwanese market. Asia‐Pacific Journal of Financial Studies, 40(5), 658-682. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[5] | Liu, H. H., Chuang, W. I., Huang, J. J. and Chen, Y. H. (2016). The overconfident trading behavior of individual versus institutional investors. International Review of Economics and Finance, 45, 518-539. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[6] | Shefrin, H. and Statman, M. (1985). The disposition to sell winners too early and ride losers too long: Theory and evidence. The Journal of Finance, 40(3), 777-790. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[7] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2000). Trading is hazardous to your wealth: The common stock investment performance of individual investors. The Journal of Finance, 55(2), 773-806. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[8] | Chen, G., Kim, K. A., Nofsinger, J. R. and Rui, O. M. (2007). Trading performance, disposition effect, overconfidence, representativeness bias, and experience of emerging market investors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(4), 425-45 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[9] | Siwar, E. (2011). The impact of overconfidence bias and disposition effect on the volume of transaction and the volatility of the French stock market. Journal of Applied Economic Sciences, 6(15), 61-83. | ||
In article | |||
[10] | Ben-David, I. and Hirshleifer, D. (2012). Are investors really reluctant to realize their losses? Trading responses to past returns and the disposition effect. The Review of Financial Studies, 25(8), 2485-2532. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[11] | Prosad, J. M., Kapoor, S., Sengupta, J. and Roychoudhary, S. (2017). Overconfidence and disposition effect in Indian equity market: An empirical evidence. Global Business Review, 19(5), 1303-1321. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[12] | Bhatia, R. and Sharma, S. (2018). Investor Overconfidence and Disposition Effect: An Evidence from India. Indian Journal of Research in Capital Markets, 5(3), 31-41. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[13] | Trejos, C., van Deemen, A., Rodríguez, Y. E. and Gomez, J. M. (2019). Overconfidence and disposition effect in the stock market: A micro world based setting. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 21, 61-69. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[14] | Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: A review of theory and empirical work. The Journal of Finance, 25(2), 383-417. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[15] | Fama. E. F. (1998). Market efficiency, long-term returns, and behavioral finance. Journal of Financial Economics, 49(3), 283-306. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[16] | Yates, J. F. (1990). Judgement and Decision Making, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. | ||
In article | |||
[17] | Campbell, W. K., Goodie, A. S. and Foster, J. D. (2004). Narcissism, confidence, and risk attitude. Journal of behavioral decision making, 17(4), 297-311. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[18] | Zaiane, S. and Abaoub, E. (2009). Investor overconfidence and trading volume: the case of an emergent market. International Review of Business Research Papers, 5(2), 213-222. | ||
In article | |||
[19] | Ouarda, M. and El Bori, A. (2014). European stock market dynamics: implications of overconfidence and the disposition effect for turnover. International Journal of Behavioral Accounting and Finance, 4(2), 133-152. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[20] | Mushinada, V. N. C. and Veluri, V. S. S. (2018). Investor’s overconfidence behavior at Bombay stock exchange. International Journal of Managerial Finance, 14 (5), 613-632. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[21] | Pompian, M. M. (2011). Behavioral finance and wealth management: how to build investment strategies that account for investor biases (Vol. 667). John Wiley & Sons. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[22] | Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P. and Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(4), 552-564. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[23] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 261-292. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[24] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2002). Does Online Trading Change Investor Behavior? European Business Organization Law Review, 3(1), 83-128. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[25] | Kadous, K., Tayler, W. B., Thayer, J. M. and Young, D. (2014). Individual characteristics and the disposition effect: the opposing effects of confidence and self-regard. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 15(3), 235-250. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[26] | Frazzini, A. (2006). The disposition effect and under-reaction to news, Journal of Finance. 61 (4), 2017-2046. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[27] | Grinblatt, M. and Keloharju, M. (2001). What makes investors trade? The Journal of Finance, 56(2), 589-616. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[28] | Cerqueira Leal, C., Rocha Armada, M. J. and Duque, J. C. (2010). Are all individual investors equally prone to the disposition effect all the time? New evidence from a small market. New Evidence from a Small Market. Frontiers in Finance and Economics, 7(2), 38-68. | ||
In article | |||
[29] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2013). The behavior of individual investors. In Handbook of the Economics of Finance (Vol. 2, pp. 1533-1570). Elsevier. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[30] | Heimer, R. Z. (2016). Peer pressure: Social interaction and the disposition effect. The Review of Financial Studies, 29(11), 3177-3209. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[31] | Komai, H., Koyano, R. and Miyakawa, D. (2018). Contrarian trades and disposition effect: Evidence from online trade data. Asian Finance Association (AsianFA) 2018 Conference. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3109297 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[32] | Hincapié-Salazar, J. and Agudelo, D. A. (2020). Is the disposition effect in bonds as strong as in stocks? Evidence from an emerging market. Global Finance Journal, 46, 100508. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[33] | Odean, T. (1998b). Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? The Journal of finance, 53(5), 1775-1798. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[34] | Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under Risk, Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[35] | De Bondt, W. F. and Thaler, R. H. (1995). Financial decision-making in markets and firms: A behavioral perspective. Handbooks in Operations Research and Management Science, 9, 385-410. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[36] | Shefrin, H. (2000). Beyond Greed and Fear. Harvard Business School Press. USA: Boston. | ||
In article | |||
[37] | Khan, I., Afeef, M., Adil, M. and Ullah, W. (2021). Behavioral factors influencing investment decisions of institutional investors: Evidence from asset management industry in Pakistan. Ilkogretim Online, 20(2), 603-614. | ||
In article | |||
[38] | Gervais, S. and Odean, T. (2001). Learning to be overconfident. The Review of Financial Studies, 14(1), 1-27. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[39] | Karpoff, J. M. (1987). The relation between price changes and trading volume: A survey. Journal of Financial and quantitative Analysis, 22(1), 109-126. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[40] | Ross, S. (1976). The arbitrage theory of capital asset pricing. Journal of Economic Theory. 13(3), 341-360. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[41] | Bessembinder, H., Chan, K. and Seguin, P. J. (1996). An empirical examination of information, differences of opinion, and trading activity, Journal of Financial Economics, 40(1), 105-134. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[42] | French, K. R., Schwert, G. and Stambaugh, R. F. (1987). Expected Stock Returns and Volatility. Journal of Financial Economics, 19(1), 3-29. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[43] | Marquardt, D. W. (1963). An algorithm for least-squares estimation of nonlinear parameters. Journal of the Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics, 11(2), 431-441. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[44] | Berndt, E. R., Hall, B. H., Hall, R. E. Hausman, J. A. (1974). Estimation and inference in nonlinear structural models. Annals of Economic and Social Measurement, 3(4). 653-665. NBER. | ||
In article | |||
Published with license by Science and Education Publishing, Copyright © 2021 Hsiang-Hsi Liu and Fei-Jen Kan
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
[1] | Odean, T. (1998a). Volume, volatility, price, and profit when all traders are above average. The Journal of Finance, 53(6), 1887-1934. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[2] | Statman, M., Thorley, S. and Vorkink, K. (2006). Investor overconfidence and trading volume. The Review of Financial Studies, 19(4), 1531-1565. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[3] | Chuang, W. I. and Lee, B. S. (2006). An empirical evaluation of the overconfidence hypothesis. Journal of Banking and Finance, 30(9), 2489-2515. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[4] | Ho, C. M. (2011). Does overconfidence harm individual investors? An empirical analysis of the Taiwanese market. Asia‐Pacific Journal of Financial Studies, 40(5), 658-682. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[5] | Liu, H. H., Chuang, W. I., Huang, J. J. and Chen, Y. H. (2016). The overconfident trading behavior of individual versus institutional investors. International Review of Economics and Finance, 45, 518-539. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[6] | Shefrin, H. and Statman, M. (1985). The disposition to sell winners too early and ride losers too long: Theory and evidence. The Journal of Finance, 40(3), 777-790. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[7] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2000). Trading is hazardous to your wealth: The common stock investment performance of individual investors. The Journal of Finance, 55(2), 773-806. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[8] | Chen, G., Kim, K. A., Nofsinger, J. R. and Rui, O. M. (2007). Trading performance, disposition effect, overconfidence, representativeness bias, and experience of emerging market investors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(4), 425-45 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[9] | Siwar, E. (2011). The impact of overconfidence bias and disposition effect on the volume of transaction and the volatility of the French stock market. Journal of Applied Economic Sciences, 6(15), 61-83. | ||
In article | |||
[10] | Ben-David, I. and Hirshleifer, D. (2012). Are investors really reluctant to realize their losses? Trading responses to past returns and the disposition effect. The Review of Financial Studies, 25(8), 2485-2532. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[11] | Prosad, J. M., Kapoor, S., Sengupta, J. and Roychoudhary, S. (2017). Overconfidence and disposition effect in Indian equity market: An empirical evidence. Global Business Review, 19(5), 1303-1321. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[12] | Bhatia, R. and Sharma, S. (2018). Investor Overconfidence and Disposition Effect: An Evidence from India. Indian Journal of Research in Capital Markets, 5(3), 31-41. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[13] | Trejos, C., van Deemen, A., Rodríguez, Y. E. and Gomez, J. M. (2019). Overconfidence and disposition effect in the stock market: A micro world based setting. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 21, 61-69. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[14] | Fama, E. F. (1970). Efficient capital markets: A review of theory and empirical work. The Journal of Finance, 25(2), 383-417. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[15] | Fama. E. F. (1998). Market efficiency, long-term returns, and behavioral finance. Journal of Financial Economics, 49(3), 283-306. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[16] | Yates, J. F. (1990). Judgement and Decision Making, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. | ||
In article | |||
[17] | Campbell, W. K., Goodie, A. S. and Foster, J. D. (2004). Narcissism, confidence, and risk attitude. Journal of behavioral decision making, 17(4), 297-311. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[18] | Zaiane, S. and Abaoub, E. (2009). Investor overconfidence and trading volume: the case of an emergent market. International Review of Business Research Papers, 5(2), 213-222. | ||
In article | |||
[19] | Ouarda, M. and El Bori, A. (2014). European stock market dynamics: implications of overconfidence and the disposition effect for turnover. International Journal of Behavioral Accounting and Finance, 4(2), 133-152. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[20] | Mushinada, V. N. C. and Veluri, V. S. S. (2018). Investor’s overconfidence behavior at Bombay stock exchange. International Journal of Managerial Finance, 14 (5), 613-632. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[21] | Pompian, M. M. (2011). Behavioral finance and wealth management: how to build investment strategies that account for investor biases (Vol. 667). John Wiley & Sons. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[22] | Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P. and Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(4), 552-564. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[23] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2001). Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 261-292. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[24] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2002). Does Online Trading Change Investor Behavior? European Business Organization Law Review, 3(1), 83-128. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[25] | Kadous, K., Tayler, W. B., Thayer, J. M. and Young, D. (2014). Individual characteristics and the disposition effect: the opposing effects of confidence and self-regard. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 15(3), 235-250. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[26] | Frazzini, A. (2006). The disposition effect and under-reaction to news, Journal of Finance. 61 (4), 2017-2046. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[27] | Grinblatt, M. and Keloharju, M. (2001). What makes investors trade? The Journal of Finance, 56(2), 589-616. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[28] | Cerqueira Leal, C., Rocha Armada, M. J. and Duque, J. C. (2010). Are all individual investors equally prone to the disposition effect all the time? New evidence from a small market. New Evidence from a Small Market. Frontiers in Finance and Economics, 7(2), 38-68. | ||
In article | |||
[29] | Barber, B. M. and Odean, T. (2013). The behavior of individual investors. In Handbook of the Economics of Finance (Vol. 2, pp. 1533-1570). Elsevier. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[30] | Heimer, R. Z. (2016). Peer pressure: Social interaction and the disposition effect. The Review of Financial Studies, 29(11), 3177-3209. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[31] | Komai, H., Koyano, R. and Miyakawa, D. (2018). Contrarian trades and disposition effect: Evidence from online trade data. Asian Finance Association (AsianFA) 2018 Conference. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3109297 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[32] | Hincapié-Salazar, J. and Agudelo, D. A. (2020). Is the disposition effect in bonds as strong as in stocks? Evidence from an emerging market. Global Finance Journal, 46, 100508. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[33] | Odean, T. (1998b). Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? The Journal of finance, 53(5), 1775-1798. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[34] | Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under Risk, Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[35] | De Bondt, W. F. and Thaler, R. H. (1995). Financial decision-making in markets and firms: A behavioral perspective. Handbooks in Operations Research and Management Science, 9, 385-410. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[36] | Shefrin, H. (2000). Beyond Greed and Fear. Harvard Business School Press. USA: Boston. | ||
In article | |||
[37] | Khan, I., Afeef, M., Adil, M. and Ullah, W. (2021). Behavioral factors influencing investment decisions of institutional investors: Evidence from asset management industry in Pakistan. Ilkogretim Online, 20(2), 603-614. | ||
In article | |||
[38] | Gervais, S. and Odean, T. (2001). Learning to be overconfident. The Review of Financial Studies, 14(1), 1-27. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[39] | Karpoff, J. M. (1987). The relation between price changes and trading volume: A survey. Journal of Financial and quantitative Analysis, 22(1), 109-126. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[40] | Ross, S. (1976). The arbitrage theory of capital asset pricing. Journal of Economic Theory. 13(3), 341-360. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[41] | Bessembinder, H., Chan, K. and Seguin, P. J. (1996). An empirical examination of information, differences of opinion, and trading activity, Journal of Financial Economics, 40(1), 105-134. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[42] | French, K. R., Schwert, G. and Stambaugh, R. F. (1987). Expected Stock Returns and Volatility. Journal of Financial Economics, 19(1), 3-29. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[43] | Marquardt, D. W. (1963). An algorithm for least-squares estimation of nonlinear parameters. Journal of the Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics, 11(2), 431-441. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[44] | Berndt, E. R., Hall, B. H., Hall, R. E. Hausman, J. A. (1974). Estimation and inference in nonlinear structural models. Annals of Economic and Social Measurement, 3(4). 653-665. NBER. | ||
In article | |||