Background: Although comprehensive knowledge of suicide risk assessment is fundamental, training programs for such an essential skill often include passive and didactic methods that may not facilitate recall. Objectives: To examine the efficacy of a recently published novel visual metaphor (VM) for teaching suicide risk factors as an adjunct to traditional teaching methods in a group of novice learners. Methods: A pilot non-blinded randomized controlled trial (RCT) was conducted. 22 first-year undergraduate psychology students were randomly assigned to either a control group, where they received traditional teaching (TT) or an intervention group, where they received traditional teaching and the visual metaphor (TT+VM). Participants then completed post-learning assessment including immediate free recall of suicide risk factors and knowledge application based on a clinical vignette. Cognitive load and participants’ satisfaction were also assessed. Descriptive statistics and the Mann-Whitney U test were used to analyse the data. Results: The TT+VM group demonstrated significantly better immediate free recall of suicide risk factors (mean= 14.56, SD= 3.2, p=.026), and superior application in the clinical vignette (mean= 14.33, SD= 1.00, p= .036). Furthermore, the TT+VM group also reported significantly less cognitive loading while learning suicide risk factors (mean= 3.44, SD= .88, p=.001) and significantly higher satisfaction levels (mean= 26.44, SD= 3.6, p=.001). The differences between the two groups on these domains ranged between medium and large effect sizes. Conclusions: Preliminary findings show that the use of TT+VM enhanced the learning of suicide risk factors. The VM could be a useful learning tool for novice learners but future large-scale studies are warranted to replicate this positive preliminary effect.
There are gaps in suicide risk assessment training 1, 2, 3 and recently published work has suggested visual metaphor (VM) as a potential way of addressing these gaps 4. For example, several studies have shown that only half of psychological trainees 5, 6 and community nurses 7 received adequate risk assessment training. Similarly, a recent study indicates that 88% of surveyed health professionals reported having received no official training in suicide risk assessment or management 8. Moreover, in non-mental health nurses dealing with high-risk groups such as oncology patients, only 1.1 % had very good knowledge about suicide and suicide assessment skills 9.
Available training programs rely heavily on didactic teaching methods which might not be the most effective way of converting knowledge about suicide risk factors into an applied clinical skill 3. A review of the effects of didactic programs and interactive education on improving clinical practice concluded that didactic programs exhibited no-to-low effects; whereas, interactive programs demonstrated moderate-to-high effects 10. VM has been proposed to help learn suicide risk factors (‘the what’), rather than how to conduct comprehensive suicide risk assessment (‘the how’) 4. VM is proposed as a means to reduce cognitive load and optimize working memory (WM) utilization, which in turn will enhance long-term memory processing 11, 12.
The purpose of this study was to assess the efficacy of VM as an adjunct to traditional methods in teaching suicide risk factors to first-year undergraduate psychology students. We hypothesized that the traditional teaching methods would be more effective in facilitating learning and recall of suicide risk factors when augmented by VM than traditional teaching methods alone.
Following ethical committee approval (MUHECN15/042), this study was conducted in 2016 at a New Zealand university. Given the novelty and nature of the intervention as an educational tool, a non-blinded pilot randomized controlled trial (RCT) design was adopted targeting novice learners of suicide risk factors.
A convenience sample of 60 first-year undergraduate psychology students were invited to participate in this trial. All participants provided written informed consents. Inclusion criteria were: (1) participants with no prior exposure to suicide risk factors teaching in tertiary education; (2) participants in their first undergraduate year of psychology, and (3) participants must be fluent in English.
2.2. Intervention OverviewThe intervention consisted of a visual metaphor of suicide risk factors. The design and interpretation of the visual metaphor is discussed in detail in previous work 4. In summary, the visual metaphor aimed to: (1) facilitate the learning of suicide risk factors; (2) improve comprehension and recall, specifically by structuring the learning content in a meaningful manner, activating prior knowledge, increasing engagement, and enhancing attention. Figure 1 depicts the visual metaphor.
Participants received sealed envelopes containing computer-generated randomized numbers allocating them to either a control group, where they received traditional teaching (TT) or an intervention group, where they received traditional teaching and the visual metaphor (TT+VM).
Participants assigned to the TT group received traditional teaching in a university classroom. Teaching content primarily focused on evidence-based suicide risk factors, though general key points about the topic such as the epidemiology of suicide were also covered. Information was presented using PowerPoint slides. The TT+VM group, on the other hand, received traditional teaching as described above, as well as the VM of suicide risk factors. The VM was first presented on a projector as a static image. The purpose and interpretation of the content in the VM was explained. Having gone through the static representation of the VM, a narrated animated video of the VM was presented to reinforce learning (available online at: https://visualmetaphoria.com/animated-video-of-the-visual-metaphor-of-suicide-risk-factors/). At the end of the learning session, participants were asked to complete a number of questionnaires. The learning session lasted for 75 minutes; 50 minutes for teaching and 25 minutes to complete the measures. Finally, each participant received a $20 voucher for participation in the trial.
2.4. MeasuresThe questionnaires that participants completed covered several domains including cognitive load, satisfaction, and memory recall. Additionally, a clinical vignette was used to assess knowledge transfer and more details are found below.
Successful memory retrieval is an indicator of effective learning 13. In order to test the participants’ immediate post-learning free recall of suicide risk factors, participants were asked to write down as many suicide risk factor as they could. For each recalled risk factor, one point was allocated, with a total maximum of 23 points.
Application of new knowledge requires knowledge transfer 14, 15 whereby successful knowledge transfer is measured by the learner’s ability to apply the newly acquired information to novel situations and problem solving 14. To measure knowledge-transfer, a clinical vignette was developed. The vignette included 17 suicide risk factors. Participants were instructed to read the vignette carefully and identify the factors that potentially increased the person’s risk of suicide. For each correctly identified risk factor, one point was allocated, with a total of 17 points. See Appendix A for the clinical vignette.
Cognitive load was measured by the Paas Cognitive Load Rating Scale 16, which assesses the amount of mental effort invested. Participants completed a 9-point Likert scale where: 1= very low mental effort, and 9= very high mental effort and this scale has shown acceptable psychometric properties 16. Recent research shows that the subjective Paas scale is equally valid as objective physiological measures such as pupillometry in measuring cognitive load 17.
The use of an existing, validated, and standardized satisfaction questionnaire was not possible because none were applicable to the context of the current study. Therefore, a satisfaction questionnaire was developed by the authors, informed by a review of satisfaction surveys found in the literature. The survey contained seven items, scored on a 5- point Likert scale where: 1 =strongly disagree and 5 =strongly agree. See Appendix B.
2.5. Statistical AnalysisData was analysed using SPSS version 23 18. Descriptive statistics were obtained and the Mann-Whitney U test was used to determine statistical significance at p=.05.
A total of 60 first-year psychology students were invited to participate and 22 agreed to be enrolled in this trial. The TT and TT+VM groups consisted of 13 and 9 participants respectively. Demographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1.
The TT+VM group achieved significantly better scores in the free recall test and the clinical vignette (Table 2). On the free recall test, participants in the TT+VM group were able to recall more suicide risk factors than those in the control group (mean= 14.56, SD= 3.2, p= .026) with a medium effect size (r=.416). Similarly, when compared to the TT group, the TT+VM group demonstrated superior application of the newly learnt information to novel situations, obtaining significantly higher scores when presented with clinical vignette (mean= 14.33, SD= 1.00, p=.036). The difference between the two groups was medium (r=.387). Furthermore, the VM caused significantly less cognitive loading on the TT+VM group’s working memory in contrast to TT group (mean= 3.44, SD= .88, p=.001) and this difference was large (r=.819). Finally, significantly greater satisfaction levels with the teaching was reported by the TT+VM group (mean= 26.44, SD= 3.6, p=.001) with a large effect size of .723.
This is the first study to assess the efficacy of a novel VM for teaching suicide risk factors as an adjunct to traditional teaching methods in a group of novice learners. Results show that the use of a VM enhanced the learning of suicide risk factors as demonstrated by significantly better performance on the post-learning free recall test and knowledge-transfer test. The VM also reduced the cognitive load imposed on participants’ WM and increased their satisfaction with the teaching significantly.
The WM is commonly regarded to hold up to seven items (plus or minus two), and process two to three items of information simultaneously 19. Effective learning occurs when information is consolidated from the WM into the long-term memory 20. However, when the amount of information presented exceeds the limits of the WM, ineffective learning occurs 21. Novice learners may find it challenging to acquire knowledge on suicide risk factors, as there are a number of static and dynamic risk factors. The VM of suicide risk factors may help overcome the limited processing and storage capacity of the WM.
In the present study, the complexity and interactivity of the content was managed as best possible. This was achieved by introducing suicide risk factors gradually, commencing with those that are potentially less complex such as the static risk factors [age and gender], and followed by the more complex dynamic risk factors. Also, the strategies by which information was presented to the learners such as distracting extraneous load was reduced. The VM included no redundant information, utilized multimodal explanatory statements (visual and auditory as appropriate, as opposed to image and written text), and presented information in an integrated format (symbols appeared in the visual metaphor sequentially and adjacent to each other).
Furthermore, the germane load (effective learning that occurs) was optimized as much as possible by several ways including schema construction 22, 23, 24. Learners hold cognitive schemas in their long-term memories. A schema is defined as domain-specific knowledge within the long-term memory 25 or as “a construct that permits us to treat multiple elements of information as a single element” 26. According to the Schema Theory, cognitive schemas allow the learner to organize related information in a meaningful way, able to be recalled later as a single unit rather than multiple elements 22, and thus reduces WM cognitive load 22, 27.
The VM of suicide risk factors, as the name implies, is a visual and metaphorical representation of risk factors. The literature shows that images and drawings of concepts result in better free recall than when expressed textually 28. This can be further explained by the Dual Coding Theory 29. The theory posits that language [verbal or text] and images are encoded and processed in the brain differently. Images of familiar objects or symbols are encoded, both visually and verbally, while text or words are encoded only verbally 30. Therefore, the chance of recalling a visual stimulus is doubled because the stimulus can be retrieved either by remembering the word or name that describes the stimulus or by the visual representation of the stimulus.
4.1. Limitations and Future DirectionsThe small sample size was an obvious limitation. A second limitation was that, although this experimental pilot RCT included random assignment and allocation concealment, blinding was not possible given that the nature of the teaching content (the presence or absence of the VM). Therefore, non-blinding might have influenced how participants responded to the questions, which in turn may have limited the validity of the study. Similarly, the absence of pre-testing to establish a baseline of participants’ prior knowledge of suicide risk factors limited the type of analysis conducted.
Future studies should aim to replicate the current study with a larger sample. This will allow for broader generalization regarding the effectiveness of the visual metaphor of suicide risk factors. It would also be useful to test the efficacy of this VM with various disciplines including psychology interns, psychologists, and senior clinicians. Investigating the impact of the VM on the learning performance of participants with different learning styles would also be useful.
Findings suggest that incorporating the VM of suicide risk factors with traditional teaching methods improved the learning of suicide risk factors. The VM of suicide risk factors could be a useful learning tool for novice learners. This preliminary finding was encouraging given the diversity, complexity, and large number of suicide risk factors. However, future large-scale studies are warranted to confirm this preliminary data.
All authors were involved in the planning of this study and in the drafting and revising of this manuscript. The execution of this study was carried out by the corresponding author.
None.
This research was supported by a Postgraduate Students Research Fund.
The authors have no competing interests.
Ethics application was approved by the university’s ethical committee (MUHECN15/042).
[1] | Cramer RJ, Johnson SM, McLaughlin J, Rausch EM, Conroy MA. Suicide risk assessment training for psychology doctoral programs: Core competencies and a framework for training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology. 2013 Feb; 7(1): 1-11. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[2] | Sudak D, Roy A, Sudak H, Lipschitz A, Maltsberger J, Hendin H. Deficiencies in suicide training in primary care specialties: A survey of training directors. Academic Psychiatry. 2007; 31(5): 345-349. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[3] | Schmitz WM, Allen MH, Feldman BN, Gutin NJ, Jahn DR, Kleespies PM, et al. Preventing suicide through improved training in suicide risk assessment and care: An American Association of Suicidology Task Force report addressing serious gaps in US mental health training. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2012; 42(3): 292-304. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[4] | Alyami M, Alyami H, Sundram F, Cheung G, Haarhoff BA, Lyndon MP, et al. Enhancing suicide risk assessment: A novel visual metaphor learning tool. Australasian Psychiatry. 2016; 1039856216657695. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[5] | Dexter-Mazza ET, Freeman KA. Graduate training and the treatment of suicidal clients: The students’ perspective. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2003; 33(2): 211-8. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[6] | Kleespies PM, Penk WE, Forsyth JP. The stress of patient suicidal behavior during clinical training: Incidence, impact, and recovery. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 1993; 24(3): 293-303. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[7] | Davies S, Amos T, Appleby L. How much risk training takes place in mental health services? A national survey of training and policies. Psychiatric Bulletin. 2001; 25(6): 217-9. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[8] | Palmieri G, Forghieri M, Ferrari S, Pingani L, Coppola P, Colombini N, et al. Suicide intervention skills in health professionals: A multidisciplinary comparison. Archives of Suicide Research. 2008 Jun 11; 12(3): 232-7. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[9] | Valente SM. Oncology nurses’ knowledge of suicide evaluation and prevention. Cancer Nursing. 2010 Jul; 33(4): 290-5. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[10] | Bloom BS. Effects of continuing medical education on improving physician clinical care and patient health: A review of systematic reviews. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care. 2005; 21(3): 380-385. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[11] | Eppler MJ. A comparison between concept maps, mind maps, conceptual diagrams, and visual metaphors as complementary tools for knowledge construction and sharing. Information Visualization. 2006 Sep 21; 5(3): 202-10. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[12] | Lakoff G, Johnson M. Metaphors we live by. 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2003. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[13] | Klemm WR. What good is learning if you don’t remember it? Journal of Effective Teaching. 2007; 7(1): 61-73. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[14] | Suebsom K, Dahalin ZM. Measuring knowledge transfer as learning outcomes using electronic media. Asia-Pacific collaborative Education Journal. 2010; 6(1): 77-97. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[15] | Lee CY, Wu FC. Factors affecting knowledge transfer and absorptive capacity in multinational corporations. The Journal of International Management Studies. 2010; 5(2): 119-126. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[16] | Paas FG. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1992; 84(4): 429-34. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[17] | Szulewski A, Gegenfurtner A, Howes DW, Sivilotti MLA, Merriënboer JJG van. Measuring physician cognitive load: Validity evidence for a physiologic and a psychometric tool. Adv in Health Sci Educ. 2016 Oct 27; 1-18. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[18] | IBM Support. Downloading IBM SPSS Statistics 23 [Internet]. 2016 [cited 2017 Apr 14]. Available from: https://www.ibm.com/support, //www.ibm.com/support/docview.wss?uid=swg24038592 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[19] | Cowan N. The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010 Feb 1; 19(1): 51-7. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[20] | Sweller J, Ayres PL, Kalyuga S. Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer; 2011. 274 p. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[21] | Schweppe J, Rummer R. Attention, working memory, and long-term memory in multimedia learning: An integrated perspective based on process models of working memory. Educ Psychol Rev. 2014 Jun 1; 26(2): 285-306. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[22] | Kirschner PA. Cognitive load theory: Implications of cognitive load theory on the design of learning. Learning and Instruction. 2002; 12(1): 1-10. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[23] | Clark RC, Nguyen F, Sweller J. Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load. San Francisco, CA: Wiley; 2011. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[24] | Qiao YQ, Shen J, Liang X, Ding S, Chen FY, Shao L, et al. Using cognitive theory to facilitate medical education. BMC Medical Education. 2014; 14(1): 79-86. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[25] | Kalyuga S, Chandler P, Tuovinen J, Sweller J. When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2001; 93(3): 579-88. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[26] | Leahy W, Sweller J. Cognitive load and the imagination effect. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2004 Nov 1; 18(7): 857-75. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[27] | Chi M, Glaser R, Rees E. Expertise in problem solving. In: Sternberg R, editor. Advances in the psychology of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1982. p. 7-75. | ||
In article | |||
[28] | Borgo R, Abdul-Rahman A, Mohamed F, Grant PW, Reppa I, Floridi L, et al. An empirical study on using visual embellishments in visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics. 2012; 18(12): 2759-2768. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[29] | Paivio A, Csapo K. Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology. 1973 Sep; 5(2): 176-206. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[30] | Paivio A. Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press; 1986. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
Based on the clinical vignette below, what are the suicide risk factors John is presenting with?
John is a 65-year-old male of a Polynesian origin, referred from the Emergency Department after attempting to end his life. John presented with low mood, sadness, loss of interests, lack of sleep, emotional dysregulation, irritability, chronic feelings of emptiness, alcohol dependence, and high blood pressure (hypertension). Along with these symptoms, John had been experiencing major problems including relationship conflicts with his family, which led to being socially isolated. He also had unstable employment and poor adherence to healthcare treatment. He had attempted to take his own life by hanging himself in the garage 2-years ago, after his wife’s death. Two weeks ago, John wrote a suicide note and planned to end his life with a gun that he had bought for this purpose. He reported feeling hopeless and not wanting to live anymore. After his recent suicide attempt, his older son, Michael, brought him to the Emergency Department. John is currently in inpatient psychiatric unit that specializes in the treatment of co-occurring disorders such as chronic depression.
Suicide risk factors are:
Please rate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each of these items by circling a value from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), where 3 indicates you neither agree nor disagree.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
[1] | Cramer RJ, Johnson SM, McLaughlin J, Rausch EM, Conroy MA. Suicide risk assessment training for psychology doctoral programs: Core competencies and a framework for training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology. 2013 Feb; 7(1): 1-11. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[2] | Sudak D, Roy A, Sudak H, Lipschitz A, Maltsberger J, Hendin H. Deficiencies in suicide training in primary care specialties: A survey of training directors. Academic Psychiatry. 2007; 31(5): 345-349. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[3] | Schmitz WM, Allen MH, Feldman BN, Gutin NJ, Jahn DR, Kleespies PM, et al. Preventing suicide through improved training in suicide risk assessment and care: An American Association of Suicidology Task Force report addressing serious gaps in US mental health training. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2012; 42(3): 292-304. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[4] | Alyami M, Alyami H, Sundram F, Cheung G, Haarhoff BA, Lyndon MP, et al. Enhancing suicide risk assessment: A novel visual metaphor learning tool. Australasian Psychiatry. 2016; 1039856216657695. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[5] | Dexter-Mazza ET, Freeman KA. Graduate training and the treatment of suicidal clients: The students’ perspective. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2003; 33(2): 211-8. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[6] | Kleespies PM, Penk WE, Forsyth JP. The stress of patient suicidal behavior during clinical training: Incidence, impact, and recovery. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 1993; 24(3): 293-303. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[7] | Davies S, Amos T, Appleby L. How much risk training takes place in mental health services? A national survey of training and policies. Psychiatric Bulletin. 2001; 25(6): 217-9. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[8] | Palmieri G, Forghieri M, Ferrari S, Pingani L, Coppola P, Colombini N, et al. Suicide intervention skills in health professionals: A multidisciplinary comparison. Archives of Suicide Research. 2008 Jun 11; 12(3): 232-7. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[9] | Valente SM. Oncology nurses’ knowledge of suicide evaluation and prevention. Cancer Nursing. 2010 Jul; 33(4): 290-5. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[10] | Bloom BS. Effects of continuing medical education on improving physician clinical care and patient health: A review of systematic reviews. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care. 2005; 21(3): 380-385. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[11] | Eppler MJ. A comparison between concept maps, mind maps, conceptual diagrams, and visual metaphors as complementary tools for knowledge construction and sharing. Information Visualization. 2006 Sep 21; 5(3): 202-10. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[12] | Lakoff G, Johnson M. Metaphors we live by. 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2003. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[13] | Klemm WR. What good is learning if you don’t remember it? Journal of Effective Teaching. 2007; 7(1): 61-73. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[14] | Suebsom K, Dahalin ZM. Measuring knowledge transfer as learning outcomes using electronic media. Asia-Pacific collaborative Education Journal. 2010; 6(1): 77-97. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[15] | Lee CY, Wu FC. Factors affecting knowledge transfer and absorptive capacity in multinational corporations. The Journal of International Management Studies. 2010; 5(2): 119-126. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[16] | Paas FG. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1992; 84(4): 429-34. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[17] | Szulewski A, Gegenfurtner A, Howes DW, Sivilotti MLA, Merriënboer JJG van. Measuring physician cognitive load: Validity evidence for a physiologic and a psychometric tool. Adv in Health Sci Educ. 2016 Oct 27; 1-18. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[18] | IBM Support. Downloading IBM SPSS Statistics 23 [Internet]. 2016 [cited 2017 Apr 14]. Available from: https://www.ibm.com/support, //www.ibm.com/support/docview.wss?uid=swg24038592 | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[19] | Cowan N. The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010 Feb 1; 19(1): 51-7. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[20] | Sweller J, Ayres PL, Kalyuga S. Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer; 2011. 274 p. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[21] | Schweppe J, Rummer R. Attention, working memory, and long-term memory in multimedia learning: An integrated perspective based on process models of working memory. Educ Psychol Rev. 2014 Jun 1; 26(2): 285-306. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[22] | Kirschner PA. Cognitive load theory: Implications of cognitive load theory on the design of learning. Learning and Instruction. 2002; 12(1): 1-10. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[23] | Clark RC, Nguyen F, Sweller J. Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load. San Francisco, CA: Wiley; 2011. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[24] | Qiao YQ, Shen J, Liang X, Ding S, Chen FY, Shao L, et al. Using cognitive theory to facilitate medical education. BMC Medical Education. 2014; 14(1): 79-86. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[25] | Kalyuga S, Chandler P, Tuovinen J, Sweller J. When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2001; 93(3): 579-88. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[26] | Leahy W, Sweller J. Cognitive load and the imagination effect. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2004 Nov 1; 18(7): 857-75. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[27] | Chi M, Glaser R, Rees E. Expertise in problem solving. In: Sternberg R, editor. Advances in the psychology of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1982. p. 7-75. | ||
In article | |||
[28] | Borgo R, Abdul-Rahman A, Mohamed F, Grant PW, Reppa I, Floridi L, et al. An empirical study on using visual embellishments in visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics. 2012; 18(12): 2759-2768. | ||
In article | View Article PubMed | ||
[29] | Paivio A, Csapo K. Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology. 1973 Sep; 5(2): 176-206. | ||
In article | View Article | ||
[30] | Paivio A. Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press; 1986. | ||
In article | View Article | ||