Article Versions
Export Article
Cite this article
  • Normal Style
  • MLA Style
  • APA Style
  • Chicago Style
Research Article
Open Access Peer-reviewed

Sex and School Differences in Executive Function Performance of Zambian Public Preschoolers

Gabriel Walubita , Beatrice Matafwali, Tamara Chansa-Kabali, Sylvia Mwanza-Kabaghe, Gershom Chongwe, Sophie Kasonde-Ng'andu, Mubanga Mofu, Janet Serenje, Adriana Bus
American Journal of Applied Psychology. 2022, 10(1), 31-39. DOI: 10.12691/ajap-10-1-5
Received September 17, 2022; Revised October 23, 2022; Accepted November 03, 2022

Abstract

Despite a great deal of research on the relations between preschool executive functions (EF) and demographic variables such as age, sex and school variables (classroom organization and teacher behaviours including quality of educational instruction, planning, organization and teacher-learner relationships), not much is known about these associations in Zambian preschoolers. This study was aimed at examining the relations of executive functions to age, sex and school variables in 133 Zambian preschool learners enrolled in urban public preschools aged 5 to 8 years. A between-subjects quasi-experimental research design was utilised. Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank model showed sex and school differences were also observed to be significantly related to executive functions, no significant relationship was found for age. Key implications of the study for pre-primary policy, girl-child education (to discourage child marriage) and preschool teacher professional development in Zambia are discussed.

1. Introduction

Sub-Saharan Africa accounts for the largest number of children who drop out of school due to child marriage and other factors 1, 2, 3 The number of children in sub-saharan Africa who fail to acquire basic reading and mathematics skills continues to remain high 4. Out of 650 million primary school children, 250 million do not have reading and mathematics skills while one-third of all primary school children who have acquired some reading skills are unable to read a sentence at the end of their primary education 4. One of the reasons for this poor performance in reading and mathematics is the lack of adequate executive functions (EF) skills 5, 6, 7. Executive functions skills are a set of three advanced cognitive skills linked to the brain's prefrontal cortex that enable a child to engage in the following skills; (1) store, update and retrieve information while engaged in other attention-demanding tasks (working memory), (2) withhold inappropriate responses (inhibitory control) and (3) switch attention to different tasks in the environment (cognitive flexibility) 8, 9. These EF skills have been found to predict several child development skills such as reading 10, maths 11, school readiness 12, responsible adult behaviour 13, social skills 14, good parenting quality 15 and non-criminal behaviour 16. Among many benefits, mastery of executive functions enables children to regulate their actions, thoughts and emotions, solve problems, make meaningful decisions, set goals and work towards goal attainment. Lack of EF skills manifests in maladjusted behaviour and inability to pay attention in class and lack of persistence on the highest pay-off tasks 17.

Factors that influence the development of EF skills include a conducive social-cultural context 18, 19, school variables such as positive classroom organization and good teacher behaviours including positive teacher instruction, planning, organization and emotional support 12. Maturation is another factor that contributes to the development of EF skills in children. Older children can perform better on cognitive tasks than their younger counterparts 20. EFs develop rapidly during preschool years 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26. Sex has also been found to be associated with executive functions. Girls tend to perform better than boys on EF tasks in Western contexts 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 33. However, the bulk of these investigations mainly focuses on high-income countries than low-income countries. Cognitive developmental milestones in children from low-income backgrounds are threatened by poor health, malnutrition and stunted growth, respiratory infections, malaria, HIV/AIDS 34, child sexual abuse 35, 36 and neurocognitive impairments 34, 37, 38. Exposure to these cognitive developmental risks has been well documented in Zambia 37, 39, 40, 41, 42.

In response to the threatened childhood cognitive development, Zambia has invested in early childhood education (ECE) to enhance early executive function development 40. Although quality early childhood education (ECE) consists of effective ECE planning and management policies, equal and wide ECE access, adequate supply of self-motivated and qualified teachers, relevant and responsive curriculum and a conducive physical environment for effective teaching and learning, Zambia falls short of these ECE requirements. Less than 15% of children have access to preschool education 43. While the urban provinces namely Lusaka (36%) and the Copperbelt (30%) account for the largest number of nursery schools' enrollments, the other provinces represent less than 20 % 44 Limited access to early childhood education in Zambia suggests that this level of education is still in its infancy.

Despite overwhelming evidence that the first 1000 days are crucial for optimal physical, cognitive, socio-emotional and moral child development, ECE investments in Zambia remain below expectation 39. Inadequately trained preschool teachers compromise quality early childhood education provision and contribute to poor learning outcomes. The victims of the failed education system in Zambia are the girl child learners in Zambia 45. The expectations are high with the new Zambian government's efforts to provide support to the education sector through the free education policy and increased teacher recruitment.

Literature has documented many factors associated with young children's executive function development, including gender, age, and academic outcomes 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51 Defined as possession of biological differences between males and females such as chromosomes (XX or XY), internal and external sex organs (ovaries, testes) and hormonal profiles (of estrogens and androgens), sex has often been found to affect brain development. Sex on the other hand refers to "socially constructed roles and relations, personality traits, attitudes and behaviours and values that are ascribed to the two sexes in a differential manner" ( 52, p. 18).

Among the most important variables to consider when examining factors related to executive functions are school variables - classroom organization and teacher behaviours including quality of educational instruction, planning, organization and teacher-learner relationships. Research in Zambia has shown that participation in a non-profit early childhood education programme for 3 to 5 hours per day predicts improved executive functions, and motor and task competence among preschool children 12. One of the plausible explanations for the significant relationship between the school environment and executive functions is drawn from the pedagogical experience hypothesis that states that teachers who provide proactive rather than reactive instructions to their learners support the development of executive functions 53. This assertion has been empirically confirmed by cross-cultural research between Chinese and American preschoolers 53.

Age has also been shown to contribute to executive functions in preschool-aged children over and above maternal education and family income in Zambia 40. This suggests that older children perform better on cognitive tasks than younger children 49, 50. Several other studies among high-income populations confirm age-related differences in executive functions both in children 22, 23 and adults 26.

Sex is one of the variables associated with executive functions in children. However, what is problematic in this field is that most of the studies conducted on the sex differences associated with EF among preschool children have been confined to Asia, America and Europe. There is consensus in the Western literature confirming that girls perform better than boys on executive function tasks among preschool children 27, 28, 29, 30. However, most of this research cannot be generalised to non-Western contexts owing to the varied child-rearing practices specific to each sex in African societies. These varied socialisation practices have resulted in different patterns of social interactions for boys and girls. For instance, girls have been observed to develop smaller and exclusive peer groups (that enhance cognitive development) as compared to boys 31. Consequently, girls tend to be relationally aggressive while boys are physically aggressive 32. These social behaviours could contribute to the sex differences in executive functions 54, 55.

Although there is substantive evidence on the development of executive functions in early childhood years 53, 56, research examining the association between executive functions and demographic factors in Zambia is limited 43. Furthermore, although, executive functions in preschool-aged children have not been extensively investigated in Sub-Saharan Africa, a huge number of children are living in deprived environments ravaged by HIV/AIDS, Diarrhoea, Malaria and respiratory infections, poverty and cognitive non-stimulation, which threaten optimal cognitive-developmental milestones on these children 39. The current study focused on investigating the relationship between demographic factors (age, school variables and gender) and executive functions among preschoolers in urban Zambian public schools.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Hundred and thirty (133) preschool learners enrolled in public schools aged between 5 to 8 years were recruited from 5 preschools. From each school, a total of 26 learners were randomly selected. The study was approved by the University of Zambia, institutional review board. Consent was first sought from the Chilanga district provincial office (PEO) and then the District Education Secretary issued an approval letter addressed to the school head teachers and teachers in the sampled schools.

Written parental consent for each of the children who participated in the study was obtained. Participants were randomly assigned to three conditions: the intervention, control 1 and 2. The intervention group spent 28 sessions spread over 6 weeks watching rhymes and stories whereas control group 1 played video games, control group 2 engaged in usual free play during the same duration.

As shown by Table 1, there were more female (73) than male (60) participants, the average age of the participants was 6.4 years with a standard deviation (SD) of 0.7.

2.2. Measures

The assessments were administered to each child individually in a quiet room at their school by trained researchers. The duration of the tests was about 17-25 min per child. While inhibitory control was assessed by the pencil tap test (PT), processing speed was measured by the Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) task.

Pencil tap Test (PT). The pencil tap task was used to measure inhibitory control, a core component of executive function, for this test the children were required to tap a pencil on the desk a predetermined number of times in response to the examiner's taps. For instance, when the examiner tapped the desk either once or twice, the child was required to respond by tapping in the opposite order of what the examiner did. The child was required to tap the desk twice (when the examiner tapped once), once (when the examiner tapped twice) and the child was required not to tap the pencil when the examiner tapped it 3 times. The correct number of taps was taken as the outcome score. Learners were tested individually by first giving them 3 practice exercises before the actual test was given. The Pencil tap test is reliable and valid for use among children from low-income families in Zambia 40 and Kenya 57. Numerous tests of inhibition for preschool children exist such as the go/no-go tasks 58. For this study, the pencil tap test was used because it is a child-friendly and culturally appropriate test with strong psychometric properties established by prior work in Zambia and Kenya 10, 40, 57.

Rapid Automatized Naming task (RAN). Processing speed was measured using Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) task 59. In this task, children were instructed to name as quickly as possible black images printed on white paper of a tree, duck, chair, pair of scissors and bicycle in a specific order. Children were first asked to identify each of the five stimuli individually to ensure that they were familiar with the objects. A practice trial was administered before the actual test was administered to ensure that the children understood the test rules. The test required the children to name the images from top to bottom and left to right as fast as they could. RAN scores have been associated with reading achievement 60, 61, 62, 63, 64. After naming 50 items in the five lines, the response time was recorded in seconds as the outcome measure. The use of the RAN in the present study was motivated by the test's established ecological validity and scalability 10, 43, 65.

2.3. Procedure

The study was conducted in the Chilanga district. Five (5) pre-schools from the district participated in the study. The preschools were selected based on a set of criteria. Preschools located within the administrative centre were included in the study for the effective movement of the research team from one school to the other considering the limited school time. The schools that met the inclusion criteria were randomly selected using simple random selection.

Participants for the present study were enrolled in the second week of the third term. Baseline assessments were administered one week before the beginning of the intervention and the same tests were re-administered in the week following the intervention.

In each preschool, three conditions (1 intervention and 2 control conditions) were created where each condition either played games (control 1) or was exposed to rhymes and stories in the local language (intervention) or engaged in usual free play (control 2). Each participant used a mobile device with headphones for maximum engagement with the material. To reduce motor strain, the devices were placed on a desk in front of the participant in an upright position (in landscape orientation) for a uniform participant experience.

The participants in the intervention condition were required to watch a 12-minute video that displayed rhymes and stories during which those in the two control conditions were required to play an action video game or engage in their usual activities outside the classroom.

Baseline. The pre-test was conducted over a period of two weeks. Each participant completed two sessions over a period of two days because the pre-test battery took approximately 45 minutes to complete. Children were assessed using the following executive function tests; the Rapid Automatised Naming test (RAN) and the pencil tap test. The procedure for test administration was standardised for all the learners. The pre-test sessions were conducted in rooms that were free from distractions.

Intervention. Participants in the intervention group spent 28 sessions spread over 6 weeks. The participants were randomly assigned to three conditions namely: intervention (watched rhymes and stories video), control 1 (played an action video game) and control 2 (engaged in free play). The Participants were exposed to 4 sessions per week and each session lasted about 12 minutes. The intervention took place within the school in a separate quiet room.

Post assessment. The participants were assessed a week after the intervention. Each participant was assessed using the Rapid Automatised Naming test (RAN) and the pencil tap test to measure executive function after exposure to the intervention.

3. Results

The goal of the present study was to examine the relations between demographic variables (school, age and gender) and executive functions in urban preschool children. The study findings will be presented as follows. First, the participant's sex and age will be present, and then the associations between executive functions and school climate and sex will be presented. Second, using the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank model, the unique contributions of the three intervention conditions (control, games and stories) to the executive function performance as measured by the two tests (Pencil tap test and Rapid Automated Naming test) will be provided and finally, the relationship between age and executive function will be shown.

3.1. Association between Executive Function (RAN) and School Variables

As shown by Table 2, There was no significant difference in the overall Rapid Automatised Naming test score between the pretest and posttest scores (p = 0.13). When the median scores were stratified by the school, the results from schools 1 and 3 showed a significantly higher difference between the pretest and posttest median scores, (p = 0.02) and 3 (p = 0.03) respectively, while other schools showed no difference. However, there was no significant difference in test scores when the results were stratified by sex.

3.2. Sex Differences in Executive Functions

Table 3 shows that there was a significant improvement in the overall executive functions as measured by the pencil tap test when the pretest and the post-test scores were compared. Similarly, there was a difference in executive function between the pretest and post-test among children in School 1 (p = 0.02) but not in the other schools. Also, the males performed better than the females on the pencil tap test (p = 0.01).

3.3. Effect of the Intervention on Executive Function Performance

As shown by Figure 1 and Figure 2, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test was used to investigate the unique contributions of the three intervention conditions (control, games and videos) to the executive function performance as measured by the two tests (Rapid Automated Naming test and Pencil tap test respectively). This set of predictors was not significantly associated with the change in EF scores on both the pencil tap and the Rapid Automatised Naming test, control (p = 0.09, 0.06) played games (p = 0.47, 0.53), and told stories (p = 0.89, 0.31).

3.4. Age-related Differences in Executive Functions

There was no correlation between age and either the pencil tap (R2 = 0.03, p = 0.72, see panel A) or the RAN (R2 = 0.02, p = 0.78, see panel B) executive function scores as shown by Figure 3.

4. Discussion

The current study examined the relations of demographic variables (age, sex and school climate) to preschool executive functions as measured by validated direct local child assessments to gain an in-depth understanding of how Zambian public preschools nurture the development of childhood executive functions. This study provided two main findings. First, while sex and school variables were associated with executive functions, age was not related to executive functions assessed in this study. Second, passive videos that the participants viewed did not predict preschool executive functions.

4.1. Age and Executive Functions

The present study found that age was not related to preschool executive functions. Contrary to this finding, Loher & Roebers 66 showed that executive function components such as sustained attention continuously improved with age from 5 to at least 8 years. These findings have also been confirmed by several other studies 67, 68, 69. It is crucial to conduct more executive function studies that consist of a wide range of age groups. There is wide consensus in the literature that age-dependent differences in executive functions among preschool children are attributed to brain maturation and development.

4.2. Sex and Executive Functions

In the current study, boys demonstrated higher executive function scores than girls partly due to specific Zambian cultural beliefs and practices and conditions that adversely affect the girl's cognitive development. With this understanding, there is no doubt that gender roles, which are constructed by society, would be related to executive functions in children. This finding is contrary to literature which reports that girls outperform boys on executive function tasks 27, 70, 71. The low performance in executive function among girls may reflect the sex differences in the risk for neurocognitive impairments in Zambia 72, The sex differences in neurocognitive development could be attributed to adverse experiences such as girl child sexual abuse, abductions, menstruation, child marriages and limited educational opportunities for girls in Zambia.

The most apparent weakness with most of the studies that have found gender-related differences in executive functions in preschool children, however, is that their samples are drawn from high-income contexts where girl children are spared from child developmental risks, which are common in low-income countries like Zambia where girl children might be subjected to child abuse, limited educational opportunities and demanding sibling care which negatively affects cognitive development 39. Lack of infrastructure for early education, inadequate teaching-learning materials, lack of trained personnel, poor funding to the education sector; poor community involvement among others makes the survival of girls difficult 65. It may be helpful to broaden the scope of Western-based studies to include non-western child populations that have inadequate quality access to education. The aim of this study attempted to address this observed deficit in Western-based research.

4.3. School Variables and Executive Functions

This study found that school participation had a significant influence on executive functions. Several prior studies substantiate the current study findings that school variables including teacher practices, teacher-child engagement, classroom climate and organization affect executive function outcomes such as inhibitory control 73, language development, mathematics and emotional development 74. However, the opposite is also true that teacher-child conflict is detrimental to cognitive development, especially adversely affecting key executive function processes including inhibitory control 75.

High-quality teacher-learner relationships such as positive emotional tone, teacher-approving behaviour and quality of instruction contribute to improved preschoolers' cognitive self-regulation 76.

One of the limitations of these studies is that they do not specify how these school outcomes were measured, such a finding is limited only to the setting investigated and cannot be easily generalised to other contexts. The association between school variables and executive functions confirms Vygotsky's theory emphasizing the role of significant knowledgeable others including teachers in stimulating child cognitive development 77. The current study proposes interventions that encourage positive teacher-learner interactions, planning and organization, classroom management, and the opportunity for learners to work freely and creatively to enhance executive functions 78.

This study was conducted to determine the associations that exist between social-demographic factors such as school, age and gender. What is clear from this study is that sex and school variables were associated with executive functions and passive videos were not effective in preschool executive functions. This study is relevant for many reasons which include the following. First, the teachers' cognitive expectations of the preschool learners and teacher beliefs contribute to the executive functions of the learner. Second, school rules which constitute the context-specific regulation and expected classroom behaviour, all affect executive functions 76. Girl child education needs to be prioritized if sex equality in access to basic social services is to be attained in Zambia.

5. Conclusion

Taken together, the contribution of this work in child neuropsychology validates the combined effect of both biological and environmental factors on executive function development in low-income preschoolers. Thus, emphasizing the biopsychosocial approach to studying cognitive development in learners. Four key implications emerge from the present study. First, the findings provide evidence that supports the Vygotskian socio-cultural theory in that the school environment contributes to improved executive functions by highlighting the need for implementation of teacher professional development programmes ineffective classroom practices and other proven preschool executive function interventions in Zambian public preschools. The associations between school variables such as classroom climate and teacher behaviours suggest that more needs to be done. For instance, the provision of quality early childhood education needs to be strengthened by motivated and qualified teachers, adequate teaching and play facilities in the preschools and teacher emotional support which have been found to predict school achievement and problem-solving in preschool-aged children.

Second, arising from the poor executive function performance on the part of girls, more effort should be directed towards girl child education for the increased provision of educational and enrichment opportunities to girls who are usually deprived of the basic cognitive stimulation experiences due to their higher risk of child sexual abuse, early marriages and poor access to education and health services 39. Girl child education intervention programmes predict enrolment and retention in Zambia 45. Third, effective parenting interventions need to be implemented in Zambia to promote holistic healthy child growth that fully supports brain maturation and development 79, 80. Additionally, the Zambian government's efforts to implement the recent Early Childhood Education (ECE) National Policy and Policy Implementation Plan, increased investment in publicly funded ECE, development of an ECE curriculum and establishment of an ECE directorate deserve support from all ECE stakeholders in Zambia. Finally, if there is a significant relationship between school variables and executive functions, the provision of high-quality preschools in low-income settings including rural areas in Zambia needs to be prioritised 43.

More progressive future research efforts should focus on the relations between parent-child engagements and executive functions in preschool learners attending private preschools which are largely unsupervised by the Ministry of General Education in Zambia. Second, research in this area needs to examine executive function interventions that are both scalable and cost-effective for low-income families in rural settings. Finally, cognitive training of executive function skills in public preschools needs to be prioritized and fully supported.

References

[1]  Kuepie, M., Shapiro, D., & Tenikue, M. (2015). Access to schooling and staying in school in selected Sub‐Saharan African countries. African Development Review, 27(4), 403-414.
In article      View Article
 
[2]  Lewin, K. M. (2009). Access to education in sub‐Saharan Africa: patterns, problems and possibilities. Comparative Education, 45(2), 151-174.
In article      View Article
 
[3]  Petroni, S., Steinhaus, M., Fenn, N. S., Stoebenau, K., & Gregowski, A. (2017). New findings on child marriage in sub-Saharan Africa. Annals of global health, 83(5-6), 781-790.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[4]  Global Education Monitoring Report. (2014). EFA Global Monitoring Report 2013/14.
In article      
 
[5]  Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[6]  Gerst, E. H., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., & Yoshida, H. (2017). Cognitive and behavioral rating measures of executive function as predictors of academic outcomes in children. Child Neuropsychology, 23(4), 381-407.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[7]  Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Wang, Y., Hillemeier, M. M., Oh, Y., & Maczuga, S. (2019). Executive function deficits in kindergarten predict repeated academic difficulties across elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 20-32.
In article      View Article
 
[8]  Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[9]  Miranda, A., Colomer, C., Mercader, J., Fernández, M. I., & Presentación, M. J. (2015). Performance-based tests versus behavioural ratings in the assessment of executive functioning in preschoolers: associations with ADHD symptoms and reading achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 545.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[10]  Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mubanga, E., Matafwali, B., Kasonde-Ngandu, S., & Bus, A. (2015). Zambian Preschools: A Boost for Early Literacy. English Linguistics Research, 4(4), 1-10.
In article      View Article
 
[11]  Yang, X., Chung, K. K. H., & McBride, C. (2019). Longitudinal contributions of executive functions and visual-spatial skills to mathematics learning in young Chinese children. Educational Psychology, 39(5), 678-704.
In article      View Article
 
[12]  McCoy, D. C., Zuilkowski, S. S., Yoshikawa, H., & Fink, G. (2017). Early Childhood Care and Education and School Readiness in Zambia. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 10(3), 482-506.
In article      View Article
 
[13]  Gray-burrows, K., Taylor, N., Connor, D. O., Sutherland, E., Conner, M., Taylor, N., … Sutherland, E. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the executive function-health behaviour relationship. Health Psychology and Behavioural Medicine, 7(1), 253-268.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[14]  Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An Integrative Framework for the Development of Social Skills, 136(1), 39-64.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[15]  Blair, C., Cybele Raver, C., & Berry, D. J. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50(2).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[16]  Murray, J., Irving, B., Farrington, D. P., Colman, I., & Bloxsom, A. J. (2010). Very early predictors of conduct problems and crime: results from a national cohort study, 11, 1198-1207.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[17]  Bierman, K., & Torres, M. (2016). Promoting the development of executive functions through early education and prevention programs. Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopmental and Translational Research, 1, 35-50.
In article      View Article
 
[18]  Fleer, M., Veresov, N., & Walker, S. (2019). Playworld's and Executive Functions in Children: Theorising with the Cultural-Historical Analytical Lenses. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[19]  Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2010). Cultural neuroscience of the self: Understanding the social grounding of the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), 111-129.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[20]  Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood.
In article      
 
[21]  Li, H., Subrahmanyam, K., Bai, X., Xie, X., & Liu, T. (2018). Viewing Fantastical Events Versus Touching Fantastical Events: Short-Term Effects on Children’s Inhibitory Control. Child Development, 89(1), 48-57.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[22]  Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The Development of Self-Regulation in the First Four Years of Life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091-1111.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[23]  Palmer, E. C., David, A. S., & Fleming, S. M. (2014). Effects of age on metacognitive efficiency. Consciousness and Cognition.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[24]  Weiss, E. M., Gschaidbauer, B., Kaufmann, L., Fink, A., Schulter, G., Mittenecker, E., & Papousek, I. (2017). Age-related differences in inhibitory control and memory updating in boys with Asperger syndrome. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(7), 651-659.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[25]  Baudouin, A., Isingrini, M., & Vanneste, S. (2019). Executive functions and processing speed in age-related differences in time estimation: a comparison of young, old, and very old adults. Ageing, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 26(2), 264-281.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[26]  Buczylowska, D., & Petermann, F. (2016). Age-Related Differences and Heterogeneity in Executive Functions: Analysis of NAB Executive Functions Module Scores. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(3), 254-262.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[27]  Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early Sex Differences in Self-Regulation and Academic Achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 689-704.
In article      View Article
 
[28]  Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(11).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[29]  Størksen, I., Ellingsen, I. T., Wanless, S. B., & McClelland, M. M. (2015). The Influence of Parental Socioeconomic Background and Sex on Self-Regulation Among 5-Year-Old Children in Norway. Early Education and Development, 26(5-6), 663-684.
In article      View Article
 
[30]  Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S. H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., … Sung, M. (2013). Sex differences in behavioural regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 621-633.
In article      View Article
 
[31]  Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Bjorkqvist, K., & Peltonen, T. (1988). Is Indirect Aggression Typical of Females? Sex Differences in Aggressiveness in 11- to 12-Year-Old Children. Aggressive Behaviour, 14, 403-414.
In article      View Article
 
[32]  Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Little, T. D., & Sawalani, G. M. (2008). Direct and Indirect Aggression During Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-Analytic Review of Sex Differences, Intercorrelations, and Relations to Maladjustment. Child Development, 79(5), 1185-1229.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[33]  Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J. A., Bilker, W. B., … Gur, R. E. (2013). Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21 Ruben. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 251-265.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[34]  Dean, O., Buda, A., Adams, H. R., Mwanza-kabaghe, S., Potchen, M. J., Mbewe, E. G., …Bearden, D. R. (2019). Pediatric Neurology Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging Findings Associated With Cognitive Impairment in Children and Adolescents With Human Immunode deficiency Virus in Zambia. Pediatric Neurology, (xxxx).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[35]  Akani, M. A., Imasiku, M. L., Paul, R., & Hazemba, A. (2015). Characteristics of child sexual abuse in Zambia. Medical Journal of Zambia, 42(4), 170-176.
In article      
 
[36]  Slonim-Nevo, V., & Mukuka, L. (2007). Child abuse and AIDS-related knowledge, attitudes and behaviour among adolescents in Zambia. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31(2), 143-159.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[37]  Adams, H. R., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mbewe, E. G., Kabundula, P. P., Potchen, M. J., Maggirwar, S., … Bearden, D. R. (2019). The HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study: Protocol of a Research Program in Pediatric HIV in Sub-Saharan Africa. Jaid.
In article      View Article
 
[38]  Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L., & Strupp, B. (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, 369(9555), 60-70.
In article      View Article
 
[39]  Hasan, R., Moucheraud, C., Bakilana, A., & Naudeau, S. (2015). Policy Brief : Zambia, (November), 1-4.
In article      
 
[40]  McCoy, D. C., Zuilkowski, S. S., & Fink, G. (2015). Poverty, physical stature, and cognitive skills: Mechanisms underlying children's school enrollment in Zambia. Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 600-614.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[41]  Mbewe, E., Kabundula, P., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mweemba, M., Buda, A., Adams, H., ... & Bearden, D. (2022). Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Function in Children with HIV: Evidence from the HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study (P1-1. Virtual).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[42]  Kabundula, P. P., Mbewe, E. G., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Birbeck, G. L., Mweemba, M., Wang, B., ... & Adams, H. R. (2022). Validation of the National Institute of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) in Children and Adolescents with and without HIV Infection in Lusaka, Zambia. AIDS and Behavior, 26(10), 3436-3449.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[43]  Zuilkowski, S. S., Fink, G., Moucheraud, C., & Matafwali, B. (2012). Early Childhood Education, Child Development and School Readiness: Evidence from Zambia. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 2(2).
In article      View Article
 
[44]  Brudevold-Newman, A., Dias, P., Folotiya, J. J., Mooya, H., Ranjit, V., & Ring, H. (2018). Study on traditional parenting and child care practices in Zambia. Retrieved from Washington, DC: https://www. unicef. org/zambia/media/1341/file/Zambia-traditional-parenting-study-2018. pdf.
In article      
 
[45]  Nkosha, C., Luchembe, M., & Chakufyali, P. N. (2013). Girl-Child Education Campaigns and Enrolment / Retention in Zambian Basic Schools: Impact Analysis. Journal of International Cooperation in Education, 15(3), 113-133.
In article      
 
[46]  Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[47]  Cortés Pascual, A., Moyano Muñoz, N., & Quilez Robres, A. (2019). The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: Review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1582.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[48]  Davidse, N. J., de Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2015). Causal relations among executive functions and academic skills from preschool to the end of first grade. English Linguistics Research, 4(1), 49.
In article      View Article
 
[49]  Dias, N. M., Menezes, A., & Seabra, A. G. (2013). Age differences in executive functions within a sample of Brazilian children and adolescents. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[50]  Gandotra, A., Csaba, S., Sattar, Y., Cserényi, V., Bizonics, R., Cserjesi, R., & Kotyuk, E. (2022). A Meta-analysis of the Relationship between Motor Skills and Executive Functions in Typically-developing Children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 23(1), 83-110.
In article      View Article
 
[51]  Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. In Measurement of Executive Function in Early Childhood (pp. 617-644). Psychology Press.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[52]  Klinge, I. (2017). Sex and sex in biomedicine. Sex and sex in biomedicine.
In article      
 
[53]  Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Purpura, D. J., Whiteman, S., Zhang, C., & Yang, F. (2019). Exploring cross-cultural variations in the development of executive function for preschoolers from low and high socioeconomic families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43(3), 212-220.
In article      View Article
 
[54]  Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2016). Sex differences in brain and behaviour in adolescence: Findings from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 159-170.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[55]  Cornblath, E. J., Tang, E., Baum, G. L., Moore, T. M., Adebimpe, A., Roalf, D. R., ... & Bassett, D. S. (2019). Sex differences in network controllability as a predictor of executive function in youth. NeuroImage, 188, 122-134.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[56]  Duh, S., Paik, J. H., Miller, P. H., Gluck, S. C., Li, H., & Himelfarb, I. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function in Chinese preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 582-591.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[57]  Brooker S, Okello G, Njagi K, Dubeck MM, Halliday KE, Inyega H, et al. (2010). Improving Educational achievement and anaemia of school children: design of a cluster-randomized trial of school-based malaria prevention and enhanced literacy instruction in Kenya. Trials., 11, 93.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[58]  Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural Inhibition, Sustained Attention, and Executive Functions: Constructing a Unifying Theory of ADHD, 121(1).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[59]  Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471-479.
In article      View Article
 
[60]  Misra, M., Katzir, T., Wolf, M., & Poldrack, R. A. (2018). Neural systems for rapid automatized naming in skilled readers: Unraveling the RAN-reading relationship. In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Reading (pp. 241-256). Routledge.
In article      View Article
 
[61]  Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(1), 1-25.
In article      View Article
 
[62]  Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual review of psychology, 63(1), 427-452.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[63]  Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G. C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2016). How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1217.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[64]  Wolff, U. (2014). RAN as a predictor of reading skills, and vice versa: Results from a randomised reading intervention. Annals of Dyslexia, 64(2), 151-165.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[65]  Matafwali, B. (2014). Evaluation of the Interactive Radio Instruction (IRI) Pilot Programme in Early Childhood Education in the Eastern Province of Zambia.
In article      
 
[66]  Loher, S., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Executive Functions and Their Differential Contribution to Sustained Attention in 5- to 8-Year-Old Children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 51-63.
In article      View Article
 
[67]  Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., Mcclelland, M. M., Morrison, J., Studies, F., … Arbor, A. (2017). The Development of Self-Regulation across Early Childhood Janelle. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1744-1762.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[68]  Perone, S., Palanisamy, J., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Age-related change in brain rhythms from early to middle childhood: Links to executive function. Developmental Science, 21(6), 1-15.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[69]  Taylor, Nicole; Enns, L. (2017). Age-Related Differences in Neuropsychological Assessment of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: A Cross-sectional Study. Biochemistry and Cell Biology.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[70]  Gestsdottir, S., von Suchodoletz, A., Wanless, S. B., Hubert, B., Guimard, P., Birgisdottir, F., … McClelland, M. (2014). Early Behavioral Self-Regulation, Academic Achievement, and Gender: Longitudinal Findings From France, Germany, and Iceland. Applied Developmental Science, 18(2), 90-109.
In article      View Article
 
[71]  von Suchodoletz, A., Gestsdottir, S., Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Birgisdottir, F., Gunzenhauser, C., & Ragnarsdottir, H. (2013). Behavioural self-regulation and relations to emergent academic skills among children in Germany and Iceland. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 62-73.
In article      View Article
 
[72]  Kabuba, N., Menon, J. A., Franklin, D. R., Heaton, R. K., & Hestad, K. A. (2016). HIV- and AIDS-associated neurocognitive functioning in Zambia - a perspective based on differences between the genders. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2021-2028.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[73]  Choi, J. Y., Castle, S., Williamson, A. C., Young, E., Worley, L., Long, M., & Horm, D. M. (2016). Teacher-Child Interactions and the Development of Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children Attending Head Start. Early Education and Development, 27(6), 751-769.
In article      View Article
 
[74]  Weiland, C., Ulvestad, K., Sachs, J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Associations between classroom quality and children’s vocabulary and executive function skills in an urban public prekindergarten program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly.
In article      View Article
 
[75]  Berry, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and teacher-child conflict: Reciprocal associations across the elementary-school years. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 66-76.
In article      View Article
 
[76]  Fuhs, M. W., Farran, D. C., & Nesbitt, K. T. (2013). Preschool classroom processes as predictors of children’s cognitive self-regulation skills development. School Psychology Quarterly, 28(4), 347-359.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[77]  Amanda P. Williford, Jessica E. Vick Whittaker, Virginia E. Vitiello, and J. T. D. (2013). Children’s Engagement within the Preschool Classroom and Their Development of Self-Regulation. Early Educ Dev, 24(2), 162-187.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[78]  Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M. D., Morrison, F. J., & Jewkes, A. M. (2008). Effects of classroom organization on letter-word reading in first grade. Journal of School Psychology, 46(2), 173-192.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[79]  Rockers, P. C., Fink, G., Zanolini, A., Banda, B., Biemba, G., Sullivan, C., … Hamer, D. H. (2016). Impact of a community-based package of interventions on child development in Zambia: A cluster-randomised controlled trial. BMJ Global Health, 1(3), 1-10.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[80]  Rockers, P. C., Zanolini, A., Banda, B., Chipili, M. M., Hughes, R. C., Hamer, D. H., & Fink, G. (2018). Two-year impact of community-based health screening and parenting groups on child development in Zambia: Follow-up to a cluster-randomized controlled trial. PLoS Medicine, 15(4), 1-15.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 

Published with license by Science and Education Publishing, Copyright © 2022 Gabriel Walubita, Beatrice Matafwali, Tamara Chansa-Kabali, Sylvia Mwanza-Kabaghe, Gershom Chongwe, Sophie Kasonde-Ng'andu, Mubanga Mofu, Janet Serenje and Adriana Bus

Creative CommonsThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Cite this article:

Normal Style
Gabriel Walubita, Beatrice Matafwali, Tamara Chansa-Kabali, Sylvia Mwanza-Kabaghe, Gershom Chongwe, Sophie Kasonde-Ng'andu, Mubanga Mofu, Janet Serenje, Adriana Bus. Sex and School Differences in Executive Function Performance of Zambian Public Preschoolers. American Journal of Applied Psychology. Vol. 10, No. 1, 2022, pp 31-39. https://pubs.sciepub.com/ajap/10/1/5
MLA Style
Walubita, Gabriel, et al. "Sex and School Differences in Executive Function Performance of Zambian Public Preschoolers." American Journal of Applied Psychology 10.1 (2022): 31-39.
APA Style
Walubita, G. , Matafwali, B. , Chansa-Kabali, T. , Mwanza-Kabaghe, S. , Chongwe, G. , Kasonde-Ng'andu, S. , Mofu, M. , Serenje, J. , & Bus, A. (2022). Sex and School Differences in Executive Function Performance of Zambian Public Preschoolers. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 10(1), 31-39.
Chicago Style
Walubita, Gabriel, Beatrice Matafwali, Tamara Chansa-Kabali, Sylvia Mwanza-Kabaghe, Gershom Chongwe, Sophie Kasonde-Ng'andu, Mubanga Mofu, Janet Serenje, and Adriana Bus. "Sex and School Differences in Executive Function Performance of Zambian Public Preschoolers." American Journal of Applied Psychology 10, no. 1 (2022): 31-39.
Share
[1]  Kuepie, M., Shapiro, D., & Tenikue, M. (2015). Access to schooling and staying in school in selected Sub‐Saharan African countries. African Development Review, 27(4), 403-414.
In article      View Article
 
[2]  Lewin, K. M. (2009). Access to education in sub‐Saharan Africa: patterns, problems and possibilities. Comparative Education, 45(2), 151-174.
In article      View Article
 
[3]  Petroni, S., Steinhaus, M., Fenn, N. S., Stoebenau, K., & Gregowski, A. (2017). New findings on child marriage in sub-Saharan Africa. Annals of global health, 83(5-6), 781-790.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[4]  Global Education Monitoring Report. (2014). EFA Global Monitoring Report 2013/14.
In article      
 
[5]  Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[6]  Gerst, E. H., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., & Yoshida, H. (2017). Cognitive and behavioral rating measures of executive function as predictors of academic outcomes in children. Child Neuropsychology, 23(4), 381-407.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[7]  Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Wang, Y., Hillemeier, M. M., Oh, Y., & Maczuga, S. (2019). Executive function deficits in kindergarten predict repeated academic difficulties across elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 20-32.
In article      View Article
 
[8]  Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[9]  Miranda, A., Colomer, C., Mercader, J., Fernández, M. I., & Presentación, M. J. (2015). Performance-based tests versus behavioural ratings in the assessment of executive functioning in preschoolers: associations with ADHD symptoms and reading achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 545.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[10]  Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mubanga, E., Matafwali, B., Kasonde-Ngandu, S., & Bus, A. (2015). Zambian Preschools: A Boost for Early Literacy. English Linguistics Research, 4(4), 1-10.
In article      View Article
 
[11]  Yang, X., Chung, K. K. H., & McBride, C. (2019). Longitudinal contributions of executive functions and visual-spatial skills to mathematics learning in young Chinese children. Educational Psychology, 39(5), 678-704.
In article      View Article
 
[12]  McCoy, D. C., Zuilkowski, S. S., Yoshikawa, H., & Fink, G. (2017). Early Childhood Care and Education and School Readiness in Zambia. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 10(3), 482-506.
In article      View Article
 
[13]  Gray-burrows, K., Taylor, N., Connor, D. O., Sutherland, E., Conner, M., Taylor, N., … Sutherland, E. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the executive function-health behaviour relationship. Health Psychology and Behavioural Medicine, 7(1), 253-268.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[14]  Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An Integrative Framework for the Development of Social Skills, 136(1), 39-64.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[15]  Blair, C., Cybele Raver, C., & Berry, D. J. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50(2).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[16]  Murray, J., Irving, B., Farrington, D. P., Colman, I., & Bloxsom, A. J. (2010). Very early predictors of conduct problems and crime: results from a national cohort study, 11, 1198-1207.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[17]  Bierman, K., & Torres, M. (2016). Promoting the development of executive functions through early education and prevention programs. Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopmental and Translational Research, 1, 35-50.
In article      View Article
 
[18]  Fleer, M., Veresov, N., & Walker, S. (2019). Playworld's and Executive Functions in Children: Theorising with the Cultural-Historical Analytical Lenses. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[19]  Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2010). Cultural neuroscience of the self: Understanding the social grounding of the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), 111-129.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[20]  Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood.
In article      
 
[21]  Li, H., Subrahmanyam, K., Bai, X., Xie, X., & Liu, T. (2018). Viewing Fantastical Events Versus Touching Fantastical Events: Short-Term Effects on Children’s Inhibitory Control. Child Development, 89(1), 48-57.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[22]  Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The Development of Self-Regulation in the First Four Years of Life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091-1111.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[23]  Palmer, E. C., David, A. S., & Fleming, S. M. (2014). Effects of age on metacognitive efficiency. Consciousness and Cognition.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[24]  Weiss, E. M., Gschaidbauer, B., Kaufmann, L., Fink, A., Schulter, G., Mittenecker, E., & Papousek, I. (2017). Age-related differences in inhibitory control and memory updating in boys with Asperger syndrome. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(7), 651-659.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[25]  Baudouin, A., Isingrini, M., & Vanneste, S. (2019). Executive functions and processing speed in age-related differences in time estimation: a comparison of young, old, and very old adults. Ageing, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 26(2), 264-281.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[26]  Buczylowska, D., & Petermann, F. (2016). Age-Related Differences and Heterogeneity in Executive Functions: Analysis of NAB Executive Functions Module Scores. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(3), 254-262.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[27]  Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early Sex Differences in Self-Regulation and Academic Achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 689-704.
In article      View Article
 
[28]  Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2016). The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 52(11).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[29]  Størksen, I., Ellingsen, I. T., Wanless, S. B., & McClelland, M. M. (2015). The Influence of Parental Socioeconomic Background and Sex on Self-Regulation Among 5-Year-Old Children in Norway. Early Education and Development, 26(5-6), 663-684.
In article      View Article
 
[30]  Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S. H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., … Sung, M. (2013). Sex differences in behavioural regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 621-633.
In article      View Article
 
[31]  Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Bjorkqvist, K., & Peltonen, T. (1988). Is Indirect Aggression Typical of Females? Sex Differences in Aggressiveness in 11- to 12-Year-Old Children. Aggressive Behaviour, 14, 403-414.
In article      View Article
 
[32]  Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Little, T. D., & Sawalani, G. M. (2008). Direct and Indirect Aggression During Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-Analytic Review of Sex Differences, Intercorrelations, and Relations to Maladjustment. Child Development, 79(5), 1185-1229.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[33]  Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J. A., Bilker, W. B., … Gur, R. E. (2013). Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21 Ruben. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 251-265.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[34]  Dean, O., Buda, A., Adams, H. R., Mwanza-kabaghe, S., Potchen, M. J., Mbewe, E. G., …Bearden, D. R. (2019). Pediatric Neurology Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging Findings Associated With Cognitive Impairment in Children and Adolescents With Human Immunode deficiency Virus in Zambia. Pediatric Neurology, (xxxx).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[35]  Akani, M. A., Imasiku, M. L., Paul, R., & Hazemba, A. (2015). Characteristics of child sexual abuse in Zambia. Medical Journal of Zambia, 42(4), 170-176.
In article      
 
[36]  Slonim-Nevo, V., & Mukuka, L. (2007). Child abuse and AIDS-related knowledge, attitudes and behaviour among adolescents in Zambia. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31(2), 143-159.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[37]  Adams, H. R., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mbewe, E. G., Kabundula, P. P., Potchen, M. J., Maggirwar, S., … Bearden, D. R. (2019). The HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study: Protocol of a Research Program in Pediatric HIV in Sub-Saharan Africa. Jaid.
In article      View Article
 
[38]  Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L., & Strupp, B. (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, 369(9555), 60-70.
In article      View Article
 
[39]  Hasan, R., Moucheraud, C., Bakilana, A., & Naudeau, S. (2015). Policy Brief : Zambia, (November), 1-4.
In article      
 
[40]  McCoy, D. C., Zuilkowski, S. S., & Fink, G. (2015). Poverty, physical stature, and cognitive skills: Mechanisms underlying children's school enrollment in Zambia. Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 600-614.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[41]  Mbewe, E., Kabundula, P., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Mweemba, M., Buda, A., Adams, H., ... & Bearden, D. (2022). Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Function in Children with HIV: Evidence from the HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorders in Zambia (HANDZ) Study (P1-1. Virtual).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[42]  Kabundula, P. P., Mbewe, E. G., Mwanza-Kabaghe, S., Birbeck, G. L., Mweemba, M., Wang, B., ... & Adams, H. R. (2022). Validation of the National Institute of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) in Children and Adolescents with and without HIV Infection in Lusaka, Zambia. AIDS and Behavior, 26(10), 3436-3449.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[43]  Zuilkowski, S. S., Fink, G., Moucheraud, C., & Matafwali, B. (2012). Early Childhood Education, Child Development and School Readiness: Evidence from Zambia. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 2(2).
In article      View Article
 
[44]  Brudevold-Newman, A., Dias, P., Folotiya, J. J., Mooya, H., Ranjit, V., & Ring, H. (2018). Study on traditional parenting and child care practices in Zambia. Retrieved from Washington, DC: https://www. unicef. org/zambia/media/1341/file/Zambia-traditional-parenting-study-2018. pdf.
In article      
 
[45]  Nkosha, C., Luchembe, M., & Chakufyali, P. N. (2013). Girl-Child Education Campaigns and Enrolment / Retention in Zambian Basic Schools: Impact Analysis. Journal of International Cooperation in Education, 15(3), 113-133.
In article      
 
[46]  Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[47]  Cortés Pascual, A., Moyano Muñoz, N., & Quilez Robres, A. (2019). The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: Review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1582.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[48]  Davidse, N. J., de Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2015). Causal relations among executive functions and academic skills from preschool to the end of first grade. English Linguistics Research, 4(1), 49.
In article      View Article
 
[49]  Dias, N. M., Menezes, A., & Seabra, A. G. (2013). Age differences in executive functions within a sample of Brazilian children and adolescents. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[50]  Gandotra, A., Csaba, S., Sattar, Y., Cserényi, V., Bizonics, R., Cserjesi, R., & Kotyuk, E. (2022). A Meta-analysis of the Relationship between Motor Skills and Executive Functions in Typically-developing Children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 23(1), 83-110.
In article      View Article
 
[51]  Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. In Measurement of Executive Function in Early Childhood (pp. 617-644). Psychology Press.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[52]  Klinge, I. (2017). Sex and sex in biomedicine. Sex and sex in biomedicine.
In article      
 
[53]  Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Purpura, D. J., Whiteman, S., Zhang, C., & Yang, F. (2019). Exploring cross-cultural variations in the development of executive function for preschoolers from low and high socioeconomic families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43(3), 212-220.
In article      View Article
 
[54]  Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2016). Sex differences in brain and behaviour in adolescence: Findings from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 159-170.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[55]  Cornblath, E. J., Tang, E., Baum, G. L., Moore, T. M., Adebimpe, A., Roalf, D. R., ... & Bassett, D. S. (2019). Sex differences in network controllability as a predictor of executive function in youth. NeuroImage, 188, 122-134.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[56]  Duh, S., Paik, J. H., Miller, P. H., Gluck, S. C., Li, H., & Himelfarb, I. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function in Chinese preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 582-591.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[57]  Brooker S, Okello G, Njagi K, Dubeck MM, Halliday KE, Inyega H, et al. (2010). Improving Educational achievement and anaemia of school children: design of a cluster-randomized trial of school-based malaria prevention and enhanced literacy instruction in Kenya. Trials., 11, 93.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[58]  Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural Inhibition, Sustained Attention, and Executive Functions: Constructing a Unifying Theory of ADHD, 121(1).
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[59]  Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471-479.
In article      View Article
 
[60]  Misra, M., Katzir, T., Wolf, M., & Poldrack, R. A. (2018). Neural systems for rapid automatized naming in skilled readers: Unraveling the RAN-reading relationship. In The Cognitive Neuroscience of Reading (pp. 241-256). Routledge.
In article      View Article
 
[61]  Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(1), 1-25.
In article      View Article
 
[62]  Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual review of psychology, 63(1), 427-452.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[63]  Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G. C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2016). How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1217.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[64]  Wolff, U. (2014). RAN as a predictor of reading skills, and vice versa: Results from a randomised reading intervention. Annals of Dyslexia, 64(2), 151-165.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[65]  Matafwali, B. (2014). Evaluation of the Interactive Radio Instruction (IRI) Pilot Programme in Early Childhood Education in the Eastern Province of Zambia.
In article      
 
[66]  Loher, S., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Executive Functions and Their Differential Contribution to Sustained Attention in 5- to 8-Year-Old Children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 3(1), 51-63.
In article      View Article
 
[67]  Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., Mcclelland, M. M., Morrison, J., Studies, F., … Arbor, A. (2017). The Development of Self-Regulation across Early Childhood Janelle. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1744-1762.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[68]  Perone, S., Palanisamy, J., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Age-related change in brain rhythms from early to middle childhood: Links to executive function. Developmental Science, 21(6), 1-15.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[69]  Taylor, Nicole; Enns, L. (2017). Age-Related Differences in Neuropsychological Assessment of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: A Cross-sectional Study. Biochemistry and Cell Biology.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[70]  Gestsdottir, S., von Suchodoletz, A., Wanless, S. B., Hubert, B., Guimard, P., Birgisdottir, F., … McClelland, M. (2014). Early Behavioral Self-Regulation, Academic Achievement, and Gender: Longitudinal Findings From France, Germany, and Iceland. Applied Developmental Science, 18(2), 90-109.
In article      View Article
 
[71]  von Suchodoletz, A., Gestsdottir, S., Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Birgisdottir, F., Gunzenhauser, C., & Ragnarsdottir, H. (2013). Behavioural self-regulation and relations to emergent academic skills among children in Germany and Iceland. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 62-73.
In article      View Article
 
[72]  Kabuba, N., Menon, J. A., Franklin, D. R., Heaton, R. K., & Hestad, K. A. (2016). HIV- and AIDS-associated neurocognitive functioning in Zambia - a perspective based on differences between the genders. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2021-2028.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[73]  Choi, J. Y., Castle, S., Williamson, A. C., Young, E., Worley, L., Long, M., & Horm, D. M. (2016). Teacher-Child Interactions and the Development of Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children Attending Head Start. Early Education and Development, 27(6), 751-769.
In article      View Article
 
[74]  Weiland, C., Ulvestad, K., Sachs, J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Associations between classroom quality and children’s vocabulary and executive function skills in an urban public prekindergarten program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly.
In article      View Article
 
[75]  Berry, D. (2012). Inhibitory control and teacher-child conflict: Reciprocal associations across the elementary-school years. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 66-76.
In article      View Article
 
[76]  Fuhs, M. W., Farran, D. C., & Nesbitt, K. T. (2013). Preschool classroom processes as predictors of children’s cognitive self-regulation skills development. School Psychology Quarterly, 28(4), 347-359.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[77]  Amanda P. Williford, Jessica E. Vick Whittaker, Virginia E. Vitiello, and J. T. D. (2013). Children’s Engagement within the Preschool Classroom and Their Development of Self-Regulation. Early Educ Dev, 24(2), 162-187.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[78]  Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M. D., Morrison, F. J., & Jewkes, A. M. (2008). Effects of classroom organization on letter-word reading in first grade. Journal of School Psychology, 46(2), 173-192.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[79]  Rockers, P. C., Fink, G., Zanolini, A., Banda, B., Biemba, G., Sullivan, C., … Hamer, D. H. (2016). Impact of a community-based package of interventions on child development in Zambia: A cluster-randomised controlled trial. BMJ Global Health, 1(3), 1-10.
In article      View Article  PubMed
 
[80]  Rockers, P. C., Zanolini, A., Banda, B., Chipili, M. M., Hughes, R. C., Hamer, D. H., & Fink, G. (2018). Two-year impact of community-based health screening and parenting groups on child development in Zambia: Follow-up to a cluster-randomized controlled trial. PLoS Medicine, 15(4), 1-15.
In article      View Article  PubMed